Sexually selected sentinels? Evidence of a role for intrasexual competition in sentinel behavior
Although the evolutionary mechanisms that favor investment in cooperative behaviors have long been a focus of research, comparatively few studies have considered the role that sexual selection may play. For example, evolutionary explanations for sentinel behavior (where 1 individual assumes an eleva...
Saved in:
Published in: | Behavioral ecology Vol. 27; no. 5; pp. 1461 - 1470 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , |
Format: | Journal Article |
Language: | English |
Published: |
United States
Oxford University Press
01-09-2016
|
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
Abstract | Although the evolutionary mechanisms that favor investment in cooperative behaviors have long been a focus of research, comparatively few studies have considered the role that sexual selection may play. For example, evolutionary explanations for sentinel behavior (where 1 individual assumes an elevated position and scans the surroundings while other group members forage nearby) have traditionally focused on the inclusive fitness benefits arising from its effects on predation risk, while its potential role in defense against intrasexual competitors remains largely unexplored. Here, we provide experimental evidence of a role for sentinel behavior in intrasexual competition, in a cooperatively breeding songbird, the white-browed sparrow weaver (
). First, dominant males sentinel substantially more than other group members (even when controlling for variation in age and body condition), consistent with a role for sentineling in intrasexual competition for mates and/or territory. Second, experimental playback of an unfamiliar male's solo song elicited a marked increase in sentineling by the dominant male, and the vocal response to the playback also positively predicted his sentinel effort following the simulated intrusion. A second experiment also suggests that sentineling may facilitate mounting rapid anti-intruder responses, as responses to intruder-playback occurred significantly earlier when the dominant male was sentineling rather than foraging at playback onset. Together, our findings provide rare support for the hypothesis that sentinel behavior plays a role in intrasexual competition, and so highlight the potential for sexually selected direct benefits to shape its expression in this and other social vertebrates. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Although the evolutionary mechanisms that favor investment in cooperative behaviors have long been a focus of research, comparatively few studies have considered the role that sexual selection may play. For example, evolutionary explanations for sentinel behavior (where 1 individual assumes an elevated position and scans the surroundings while other group members forage nearby) have traditionally focused on the inclusive fitness benefits arising from its effects on predation risk, while its potential role in defense against intrasexual competitors remains largely unexplored. Here, we provide experimental evidence of a role for sentinel behavior in intrasexual competition, in a cooperatively breeding songbird, the white-browed sparrow weaver (
). First, dominant males sentinel substantially more than other group members (even when controlling for variation in age and body condition), consistent with a role for sentineling in intrasexual competition for mates and/or territory. Second, experimental playback of an unfamiliar male's solo song elicited a marked increase in sentineling by the dominant male, and the vocal response to the playback also positively predicted his sentinel effort following the simulated intrusion. A second experiment also suggests that sentineling may facilitate mounting rapid anti-intruder responses, as responses to intruder-playback occurred significantly earlier when the dominant male was sentineling rather than foraging at playback onset. Together, our findings provide rare support for the hypothesis that sentinel behavior plays a role in intrasexual competition, and so highlight the potential for sexually selected direct benefits to shape its expression in this and other social vertebrates. Few studies have considered whether competition between members of the same sex has shaped the expression of cooperative behaviors. Here, we provide experimental evidence that sentinel behavior plays a role in defense against same-sex competitors. Song playbacks that simulated male intrusions elicited a marked increase in sentineling by the dominant male, and also suggested that the sentinel position itself may facilitate the initiation of anti-intruder responses. Twitter: @Linds__Walker @animalsocieties Although the evolutionary mechanisms that favor investment in cooperative behaviors have long been a focus of research, comparatively few studies have considered the role that sexual selection may play. For example, evolutionary explanations for sentinel behavior (where 1 individual assumes an elevated position and scans the surroundings while other group members forage nearby) have traditionally focused on the inclusive fitness benefits arising from its effects on predation risk, while its potential role in defense against intrasexual competitors remains largely unexplored. Here, we provide experimental evidence of a role for sentinel behavior in intrasexual competition, in a cooperatively breeding songbird, the white-browed sparrow weaver ( Plocepasser mahali ). First, dominant males sentinel substantially more than other group members (even when controlling for variation in age and body condition), consistent with a role for sentineling in intrasexual competition for mates and/or territory. Second, experimental playback of an unfamiliar male’s solo song elicited a marked increase in sentineling by the dominant male, and the vocal response to the playback also positively predicted his sentinel effort following the simulated intrusion. A second experiment also suggests that sentineling may facilitate mounting rapid anti-intruder responses, as responses to intruder-playback occurred significantly earlier when the dominant male was sentineling rather than foraging at playback onset. Together, our findings provide rare support for the hypothesis that sentinel behavior plays a role in intrasexual competition, and so highlight the potential for sexually selected direct benefits to shape its expression in this and other social vertebrates. |
Author | Young, Andrew J Walker, Lindsay A York, Jenny E |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Lindsay A surname: Walker fullname: Walker, Lindsay A organization: Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter , Cornwall Campus, Treliever Road, Penryn, Cornwall TR10 9EZ , UK and – sequence: 2 givenname: Jenny E surname: York fullname: York, Jenny E organization: Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, Cornwall Campus, Treliever Road, Penryn, Cornwall TR10 9EZ, UK and; Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, UK – sequence: 3 givenname: Andrew J surname: Young fullname: Young, Andrew J organization: Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter , Cornwall Campus, Treliever Road, Penryn, Cornwall TR10 9EZ , UK and |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27656086$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNpVUbtOAzEQtFAQJEBJi1zSHNjn110DQhEvCYkCqI3P2QMjxw72JZC_50IggmpHu7Ozo50RGoQYAKFDSk4oqdlpA69g46lJH0TyLTSkXIpClaoe9JhwUZQlr3fRKOc3QgitudxBu6WSQpJKDtHzA3zOjfdLnMGD7WDSg9C5AD6f48uFm0CwgGOLDU7RA25jwi50yeTvRWzjdAad61wMfX-zjHtfZuFi2kfbrfEZDn7qHnq6unwc3xR399e344u7wrJKdUXbcFJWraCqAcIqkIbLkltCZSUIZ1LUjepRLUFUjAI0VJpGKUOgspw0jO2hs7XubN5MYWJh5dHrWXJTk5Y6Gqf_T4J71S9xoQXpv1GuBI5_BFJ8n0Pu9NRlC96bAHGeNa2EYKUSgvbUYk21KeacoN2coUSvUtHrVPQ6lZ5_9Nfbhv0bA_sChuSOQQ |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1111_mec_15868 crossref_primary_10_1242_jeb_247003 crossref_primary_10_7554_eLife_74550 crossref_primary_10_1111_ibi_12956 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2020_04_021 crossref_primary_10_1098_rspb_2023_0901 crossref_primary_10_3390_biology11121769 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2021_01_002 crossref_primary_10_1111_eth_12833 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pbio_3002356 crossref_primary_10_1111_mec_15181 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00265_023_03403_w crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2019_10_001 crossref_primary_10_1126_sciadv_abe8980 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_anbehav_2019_09_018 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_160738 crossref_primary_10_1093_beheco_arz095 |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.10.012 10.1017/CBO9780511606816.010 10.1098/rspb.1999.0737 10.1046/j.0179-1613.2003.00926.x 10.1016/0003-3472(89)90144-9 10.1046/j.0962-1083.2001.01355.x 10.1111/mec.12505 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0023 10.1163/156853906775900739 10.1016/j.cub.2009.02.036 10.1093/beheco/arr021 10.1098/rstb.2013.0075 10.1002/ece3.1938 10.1007/s00265-013-1599-1 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0970 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1982.tb03795.x 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.029 10.2989/OSTRICH.2009.80.2.6.833 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.10.017 10.1007/978-0-387-21706-2 10.2307/3677250 10.1126/science.284.5420.1640 10.1098/rspb.2007.0316 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01180.x 10.1111/mec.12978 10.1017/CBO9780511606816.004 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.033 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1994.tb01034.x 10.1038/nature05386 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0844 10.1006/anbe.2001.1838 10.2307/3677205 10.1017/CBO9780511752452.005 10.1006/anbe.1993.1001 10.1046/j.0021-8790.2001.00565.x 10.1111/1365-2656.12057 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.17643.x 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00598.x 10.1017/CBO9780511641954.016 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80104-6 10.1080/00306525.1988.9633696 10.1098/rspb.2000.1574 10.1111/1365-2435.12317 10.1098/rspb.2010.0643 10.1126/science.1071412 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.05.021 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1987.tb00685.x 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01258.x 10.1017/CBO9780511752452.018 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | The Author 2016. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology. 2016 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: The Author 2016. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology. 2016 |
DBID | NPM AAYXX CITATION 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.1093/beheco/arw064 |
DatabaseName | PubMed CrossRef MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | PubMed CrossRef MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | PubMed |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Zoology Biology |
EISSN | 1465-7279 |
EndPage | 1470 |
ExternalDocumentID | 10_1093_beheco_arw064 27656086 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council grantid: BB/H022716/1 – fundername: University of Pretoria |
GroupedDBID | -E4 .2P .I3 0R~ 1TH 1VV 23N 2WC 4.4 482 48X 5GY 5VS 5WA 5WD 6.Y 70D 9M8 AAHBH AAIMJ AAJKP AAJQQ AAMDB AAMVS AAOGV AAPQZ AAPXW AARHZ AAUAY AAUQX AAVAP AAVLN AAWDT AAYJJ ABEUO ABIVO ABIXL ABJNI ABMNT ABNKS ABPTD ABQLI ABQTQ ABSAR ABSMQ ABWST ABXVV ABZBJ ACFRR ACGFS ACGOD ACIPB ACIWK ACMRT ACNCT ACPQN ACPRK ACUFI ACUTJ ACZBC ADBBV ADEYI ADEZT ADFTL ADGKP ADGZP ADHKW ADHZD ADIPN ADOCK ADQBN ADRIX ADRTK ADVEK ADYVW ADZTZ ADZXQ AEGPL AEHUL AEJOX AEKPW AEKSI AELWJ AEMDU AENEX AENZO AEPUE AETBJ AEWNT AFFZL AFGWE AFIYH AFOFC AFRAH AFSHK AFXEN AFYAG AGINJ AGKEF AGKRT AGMDO AGQXC AGSYK AHXPO AI. AIJHB AJEEA AKHUL AKWXX ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALUQC ANFBD APIBT APJGH APWMN AQDSO ARIXL ASAOO ASPBG ATDFG ATGXG ATTQO AVWKF AXUDD AYOIW AZFZN BAYMD BCRHZ BEYMZ BHONS BQDIO BSWAC C1A CAG CDBKE COF CS3 CXTWN CZ4 D0L DAKXR DFGAJ DILTD DU5 D~K E3Z EBS ECGQY EE~ EJD ELUNK F5P F9B FEDTE FHSFR FLUFQ FOEOM FQBLK GAUVT GJXCC H13 H5~ HAR HVGLF HW0 HZ~ H~9 IOX J21 KAQDR KBUDW KOP KQ8 KSI KSN M-Z M49 MBTAY ML0 MVM N9A NGC NLBLG NOMLY NPM NTWIH NU- NVLIB O0~ O9- OAWHX OBOKY ODMLO OHT OJQWA OJZSN OK1 OVD OWPYF O~Y P2P PAFKI PB- PEELM PQQKQ Q1. Q5Y QBD R44 RD5 RIG RNI ROL ROX ROZ RUSNO RW1 RXO RZF RZO TCN TEORI TLC TR2 TWZ VH1 W8F WH7 WOQ X7H XIH YAYTL YKOAZ YXANX ZCA ZKX ~02 ~91 AAYXX ABEJV CITATION 7X8 5PM AASNB |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c387t-fb4028f517be038e6a4624c01685043659b785096e5831eeb16ab77a0e8c40b33 |
ISSN | 1045-2249 |
IngestDate | Tue Sep 17 21:20:43 EDT 2024 Fri Oct 25 02:20:16 EDT 2024 Fri Nov 22 00:04:16 EST 2024 Wed Oct 16 00:42:22 EDT 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 5 |
Keywords | sentinels vigilance intrasexual competition Plocepasser mahali sexual selection |
Language | English |
License | This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c387t-fb4028f517be038e6a4624c01685043659b785096e5831eeb16ab77a0e8c40b33 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 Handling editor: Louise Barrett |
OpenAccessLink | https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC5027623 |
PMID | 27656086 |
PQID | 1855327551 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 10 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_5027623 proquest_miscellaneous_1855327551 crossref_primary_10_1093_beheco_arw064 pubmed_primary_27656086 |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2016-09-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2016-09-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 09 year: 2016 text: 2016-09-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States – name: UK |
PublicationTitle | Behavioral ecology |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Behav Ecol |
PublicationYear | 2016 |
Publisher | Oxford University Press |
Publisher_xml | – name: Oxford University Press |
References | 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.38 Hailman (2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.15) 1994; 97 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.39 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.32 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.33 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.30 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.31 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.36 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.37 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.34 Beynon (2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.4) 1989; 85 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.35 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.40 York (2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.48) 2014; 10 Collias (2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.9) 1978; 95 Leitner (2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.21) 2009; 80 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.49 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.43 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.44 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.41 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.42 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.47 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.45 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.46 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.50 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.51 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.18 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.19 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.16 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.17 Rasa (2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.29) 1986; 71 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.2 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.10 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.54 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.1 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.11 Ferguson (2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.13) 1987; 76 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.52 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.53 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.14 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.12 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.8 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.7 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.6 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.5 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.3 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.27 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.28 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.22 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.20 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.25 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.26 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.23 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.24 |
References_xml | – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.19 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.10.012 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.20 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511606816.010 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.23 doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0737 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.2 doi: 10.1046/j.0179-1613.2003.00926.x – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.26 doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(89)90144-9 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.32 doi: 10.1046/j.0962-1083.2001.01355.x – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.17 doi: 10.1111/mec.12505 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.34 doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0023 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.39 doi: 10.1163/156853906775900739 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.36 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.02.036 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.24 doi: 10.1093/beheco/arr021 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.50 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0075 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.47 doi: 10.1002/ece3.1938 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.16 doi: 10.1007/s00265-013-1599-1 – volume: 10 start-page: 1 year: 2014 ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.48 article-title: Singing in the moonlight: dawn song performance of a diurnal bird varies with lunar phase publication-title: Biol Lett doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0970 contributor: fullname: York – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.22 doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1982.tb03795.x – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.35 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.029 – volume: 85 start-page: 447 year: 1989 ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.4 article-title: Do dwarf mongooses have a language? Warning vocalizations transmit complex information publication-title: S Afr J Sci contributor: fullname: Beynon – volume: 80 start-page: 99 year: 2009 ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.21 article-title: Morphometrics of white-browed sparrow-weavers Plocepasser mahali in south western Zimbabwe publication-title: Ostrich doi: 10.2989/OSTRICH.2009.80.2.6.833 contributor: fullname: Leitner – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.33 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.10.017 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.38 doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-21706-2 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.42 doi: 10.2307/3677250 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.6 doi: 10.1126/science.284.5420.1640 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.51 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.0316 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.28 doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01180.x – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.18 doi: 10.1111/mec.12978 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.11 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511606816.004 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.46 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.5 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.033 – volume: 97 start-page: 119 year: 1994 ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.15 article-title: Role of helpers in the sentinel behaviour of the Florida scrub jay (Aphelocoma c. coerulescens) publication-title: Ethology doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1994.tb01034.x contributor: fullname: Hailman – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.7 doi: 10.1038/nature05386 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.37 doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2011.0844 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.43 doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1838 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.53 doi: 10.2307/3677205 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.52 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511752452.005 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.41 doi: 10.1006/anbe.1993.1001 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.44 doi: 10.1046/j.0021-8790.2001.00565.x – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.54 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.12 doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12057 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.27 doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.17643.x – volume: 71 start-page: 340 year: 1986 ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.29 article-title: Coordinated vigilance in dwarf mongoose family groups: the “Watchman’s Song” hypothesis and the costs of guarding publication-title: Ethology doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00598.x contributor: fullname: Rasa – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.49 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511641954.016 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.1 doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80104-6 – volume: 95 start-page: 472 year: 1978 ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.9 article-title: Cooperative breeding behaviour in the white-browed sparrow weaver publication-title: Auk contributor: fullname: Collias – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.14 doi: 10.1080/00306525.1988.9633696 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.45 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1574 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.10 doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12317 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.3 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0643 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.8 doi: 10.1126/science.1071412 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.25 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.05.021 – volume: 76 start-page: 223 year: 1987 ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.13 article-title: Vigilance behaviour in white-browed sparrow-weavers Plocepasser mahali publication-title: Ethology doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1987.tb00685.x contributor: fullname: Ferguson – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.30 – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.40 doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01258.x – ident: 2016091907481008000_27.5.1461.31 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511752452.018 |
SSID | ssj0001946 |
Score | 2.3164551 |
Snippet | Although the evolutionary mechanisms that favor investment in cooperative behaviors have long been a focus of research, comparatively few studies have... Few studies have considered whether competition between members of the same sex has shaped the expression of cooperative behaviors. Here, we provide... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest crossref pubmed |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database |
StartPage | 1461 |
SubjectTerms | Original |
Title | Sexually selected sentinels? Evidence of a role for intrasexual competition in sentinel behavior |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27656086 https://search.proquest.com/docview/1855327551 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC5027623 |
Volume | 27 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://sdu.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1La9wwEBZNQqGX0ne3TYsKbS_BRLash08lj11yCOkhm7D04vohk0LwlvUuZf99ZyxLtpNLeujFGO3K2tV8jD7JM98Q8lkjyxActyVCBnHFRJAAbQ6SkpWhTAxXbSLt2aW6WOjTaTztc0_6tv9qaWgDW2Pm7D9Y2z8UGuAebA5XsDpcH2T3y1ZD-XZ70LQVboBPYnoRcMnb5gufHbgqojYvso0trNqQ9PUK1jPs2kaZG6thhKchrrvP6B-9B-6z_E1x94S-C9jAbX-TbftTU1f5ByNrtn0qhHc8Nsiye2HVnUiE0odcOScKNDEAapAMvaxVAOjQJAYuEwuLD5bfMLaFRO65dit7BX8V_g4uYKs_zOqfj0W0L76ns6vz83Q-Xcx3yF4E_gfc397R8eL62i_RYWLTztwP7cRXYYhDO8ChffyYrNzbgdwNpB0wk_kz8rTbUtAji4Xn5JGpX5DHtsjoFu5-LNu7l-SnwwZ12KAeG9-oQwZdVjSjiAwKyKADZNABMqDdd6YOGa_I1Ww6PzkLugobQcG1WgdVHgO_rESocsO4NjKLZRQXYFONynZSJLnSKBBkhOahgXVdZrlSGTO6iFnO-WuyWy9r85bQxCghVWmAQAIxKllSyUJXuFvXLDZCTshXN5XpbyukktoACJ7aOU_tnE_IJzfRKbg6fH-V1Wa5aVKgloJHCjj-hLyxE-8fFSlUkdIwjBqZxH8BZdTHn9S_blo5dcGgc8TfPWDc9-RJD_h9srtebcwHstOUm48dxP4C8MeTXQ |
link.rule.ids | 230,315,782,786,887,27933,27934 |
linkProvider | Oxford University Press |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Sexually+selected+sentinels%3F+Evidence+of+a+role+for+intrasexual+competition+in+sentinel+behavior&rft.jtitle=Behavioral+ecology&rft.au=Walker%2C+Lindsay+A&rft.au=York%2C+Jenny+E&rft.au=Young%2C+Andrew+J&rft.date=2016-09-01&rft.issn=1045-2249&rft.volume=27&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=1461&rft.epage=1470&rft_id=info:doi/10.1093%2Fbeheco%2Farw064&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1045-2249&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1045-2249&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1045-2249&client=summon |