Context-specific adaptation for head fakes in basketball: a study on player-specific fake-frequency schedules

In basketball, an attacking player often plays a pass to one side while looking to the other side. This head fake provokes a conflict in the observing opponent, as the processing of the head orientation interferes with the processing of the pass direction. Accordingly, responses to passes with head...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Psychological research Vol. 88; no. 5; pp. 1702 - 1711
Main Authors: Güldenpenning, Iris, Böer, Nils T., Kunde, Wilfried, Giesen, Carina G., Rothermund, Klaus, Weigelt, Matthias
Format: Journal Article
Language:English
Published: Berlin/Heidelberg Springer Berlin Heidelberg 01-07-2024
Springer Nature B.V
Subjects:
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
Abstract In basketball, an attacking player often plays a pass to one side while looking to the other side. This head fake provokes a conflict in the observing opponent, as the processing of the head orientation interferes with the processing of the pass direction. Accordingly, responses to passes with head fakes are slower and result in more errors than responses to passes without head fakes (head-fake effect). The head-fake effect and structurally similar interference effects (e.g., Stroop effect) are modulated by the frequency of conflicting trials. Previous studies mostly applied a block-wise manipulation of proportion congruency. However, in basketball (and also in other team sports), where different individual opponents can be encountered, it might be important to take the individual frequency (e.g., 20% vs. 80%) of these opponents into account. Therefore, the present study investigates the possibility to quickly (i.e., on a trial-by-trial basis) reconfigure the response behavior to different proportions of incongruent trials, which are contingent on different basketball players. Results point out that participants indeed adapted to the fake-frequency of different basketball players, which could be the result of strategic adaptation processes. Multi-level analyses, however, indicate that a substantial portion of the player-specific adaptation to fake frequencies is accounted by episodic retrieval processes, suggesting that item-specific proportion congruency effects can be explained in terms of stimulus-response binding and retrieval: The head orientation (e.g., to the right) of a current stimulus retrieves the last episode with the same head orientation including the response that was part of this last episode. Thus, from a theoretical perspective, an attacking player would provoke the strongest detrimental effect on an opponent if s/he repeats the same head movement but changes the direction of the pass. Whether it is at all possible to strategically apply this recommendation in practice needs still to be answered.
AbstractList In basketball, an attacking player often plays a pass to one side while looking to the other side. This head fake provokes a conflict in the observing opponent, as the processing of the head orientation interferes with the processing of the pass direction. Accordingly, responses to passes with head fakes are slower and result in more errors than responses to passes without head fakes (head-fake effect). The head-fake effect and structurally similar interference effects (e.g., Stroop effect) are modulated by the frequency of conflicting trials. Previous studies mostly applied a block-wise manipulation of proportion congruency. However, in basketball (and also in other team sports), where different individual opponents can be encountered, it might be important to take the individual frequency (e.g., 20% vs. 80%) of these opponents into account. Therefore, the present study investigates the possibility to quickly (i.e., on a trial-by-trial basis) reconfigure the response behavior to different proportions of incongruent trials, which are contingent on different basketball players. Results point out that participants indeed adapted to the fake-frequency of different basketball players, which could be the result of strategic adaptation processes. Multi-level analyses, however, indicate that a substantial portion of the player-specific adaptation to fake frequencies is accounted by episodic retrieval processes, suggesting that item-specific proportion congruency effects can be explained in terms of stimulus-response binding and retrieval: The head orientation (e.g., to the right) of a current stimulus retrieves the last episode with the same head orientation including the response that was part of this last episode. Thus, from a theoretical perspective, an attacking player would provoke the strongest detrimental effect on an opponent if s/he repeats the same head movement but changes the direction of the pass. Whether it is at all possible to strategically apply this recommendation in practice needs still to be answered.
In basketball, an attacking player often plays a pass to one side while looking to the other side. This head fake provokes a conflict in the observing opponent, as the processing of the head orientation interferes with the processing of the pass direction. Accordingly, responses to passes with head fakes are slower and result in more errors than responses to passes without head fakes (head-fake effect). The head-fake effect and structurally similar interference effects (e.g., Stroop effect) are modulated by the frequency of conflicting trials. Previous studies mostly applied a block-wise manipulation of proportion congruency. However, in basketball (and also in other team sports), where different individual opponents can be encountered, it might be important to take the individual frequency (e.g., 20% vs. 80%) of these opponents into account. Therefore, the present study investigates the possibility to quickly (i.e., on a trial-by-trial basis) reconfigure the response behavior to different proportions of incongruent trials, which are contingent on different basketball players. Results point out that participants indeed adapted to the fake-frequency of different basketball players, which could be the result of strategic adaptation processes. Multi-level analyses, however, indicate that a substantial portion of the player-specific adaptation to fake frequencies is accounted by episodic retrieval processes, suggesting that item-specific proportion congruency effects can be explained in terms of stimulus-response binding and retrieval: The head orientation (e.g., to the right) of a current stimulus retrieves the last episode with the same head orientation including the response that was part of this last episode. Thus, from a theoretical perspective, an attacking player would provoke the strongest detrimental effect on an opponent if s/he repeats the same head movement but changes the direction of the pass. Whether it is at all possible to strategically apply this recommendation in practice needs still to be answered.In basketball, an attacking player often plays a pass to one side while looking to the other side. This head fake provokes a conflict in the observing opponent, as the processing of the head orientation interferes with the processing of the pass direction. Accordingly, responses to passes with head fakes are slower and result in more errors than responses to passes without head fakes (head-fake effect). The head-fake effect and structurally similar interference effects (e.g., Stroop effect) are modulated by the frequency of conflicting trials. Previous studies mostly applied a block-wise manipulation of proportion congruency. However, in basketball (and also in other team sports), where different individual opponents can be encountered, it might be important to take the individual frequency (e.g., 20% vs. 80%) of these opponents into account. Therefore, the present study investigates the possibility to quickly (i.e., on a trial-by-trial basis) reconfigure the response behavior to different proportions of incongruent trials, which are contingent on different basketball players. Results point out that participants indeed adapted to the fake-frequency of different basketball players, which could be the result of strategic adaptation processes. Multi-level analyses, however, indicate that a substantial portion of the player-specific adaptation to fake frequencies is accounted by episodic retrieval processes, suggesting that item-specific proportion congruency effects can be explained in terms of stimulus-response binding and retrieval: The head orientation (e.g., to the right) of a current stimulus retrieves the last episode with the same head orientation including the response that was part of this last episode. Thus, from a theoretical perspective, an attacking player would provoke the strongest detrimental effect on an opponent if s/he repeats the same head movement but changes the direction of the pass. Whether it is at all possible to strategically apply this recommendation in practice needs still to be answered.
Author Kunde, Wilfried
Weigelt, Matthias
Rothermund, Klaus
Giesen, Carina G.
Böer, Nils T.
Güldenpenning, Iris
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Iris
  orcidid: 0000-0003-0549-5543
  surname: Güldenpenning
  fullname: Güldenpenning, Iris
  email: iris.gueldenpenning@uni-paderborn.de
  organization: Department of Sport & Health, University of Paderborn
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Nils T.
  orcidid: 0000-0002-0236-7282
  surname: Böer
  fullname: Böer, Nils T.
  organization: Department of Sport & Health, University of Paderborn
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Wilfried
  orcidid: 0000-0001-6256-8011
  surname: Kunde
  fullname: Kunde, Wilfried
  organization: Department of Psychology, Würzburg University
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Carina G.
  orcidid: 0000-0002-2395-4435
  surname: Giesen
  fullname: Giesen, Carina G.
  organization: General Psychology, Health & Medical University
– sequence: 5
  givenname: Klaus
  orcidid: 0000-0002-9350-5272
  surname: Rothermund
  fullname: Rothermund, Klaus
  organization: Department of General Psychology II, Friedrich Schiller University Jena
– sequence: 6
  givenname: Matthias
  orcidid: 0000-0002-0348-9097
  surname: Weigelt
  fullname: Weigelt, Matthias
  organization: Department of Sport & Health, University of Paderborn
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38806734$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9kb1uFDEUhS0URDaBF6BAlmhoDP6bsU2D0Io_KRIN1JbHc52dZNYebA9i3x6HDQlQULm43z3XR98ZOokpAkJPGX3JKFWvCqWS94RySSgzShH-AG2YFJRwpfgJ2lAhKaGKq1N0VsoVpUz1vXqEToXWtFdCbtB-m2KFH5WUBfwUJo_d6Jbq6pQiDinjHbgRB3cNBU8RD65cQx3cPL_GDpe6jgfcwGV2B8j3GTc8CRm-rRD9ARe_g3GdoTxGD4ObCzy5fc_R1_fvvmw_kovPHz5t314QL7q-EuaoENwPgQU_gGGj7Lh0SvQq8FZncIEpEKD1MErlZTDcGdN3XI_S6KHz4hy9OeYu67CH0UOs2c12ydPe5YNNbrJ_T-K0s5fpu2WMa2Y62RJe3Cbk1FqUavdT8TDPLkJaixW0Z0pLY1hDn_-DXqU1x9avUbprkpqTRvEj5XMqJUO4-w2j9kanPeq0Taf9pdPytvTszx53K7_9NUAcgdJG8RLy_e3_xP4EMm-uYg
Cites_doi 10.1016/j.concog.2009.08.009
10.3758/s13423-011-0119-4
10.1037/xlm0001353
10.1177/1747021818787155
10.1002/9781118920497.ch5
10.3758/BF03193850
10.1026/1612-5010/a000075
10.1016/j.tics.2020.02.004
10.3389/fpsyg.2020.592377
10.1037/cep0000066
10.3758/PBR.15.4.814
10.1177/1747021820906397
10.1080/1612197x.2020.1854819
10.1371/journal.pone.0251117
10.1016/j.concog.2006.06.010
10.5334/joc.227
10.1016/j.tics.2019.07.002
10.1037/a0029794
10.3758/BF03196526
10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.11.010
10.3758/s13428-012-0186-0
10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02927
10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00367
10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.480
10.1007/s00426-018-1078-4
10.3758/s13423-016-1140-4
10.1080/17470218.2016.1182193
10.3758/s13414-022-02447-w
10.1037/a0036437
10.1007/s12662-016-0401-8
10.3758/BF03196448
10.1037/h0080661
10.5334/joc.232
10.3758/BF03197535
10.1007/s12662-018-0493-4
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright The Author(s) 2024
2024. The Author(s).
The Author(s) 2024. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
The Author(s) 2024 2024
Copyright_xml – notice: The Author(s) 2024
– notice: 2024. The Author(s).
– notice: The Author(s) 2024. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
– notice: The Author(s) 2024 2024
DBID C6C
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
AAYXX
CITATION
7TK
K9.
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.1007/s00426-024-01977-2
DatabaseName SpringerOpen
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
CrossRef
Neurosciences Abstracts
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
CrossRef
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList CrossRef
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
MEDLINE

MEDLINE - Academic

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: ECM
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=cmedm&site=ehost-live
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 1430-2772
EndPage 1711
ExternalDocumentID 10_1007_s00426_024_01977_2
38806734
Genre Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Universität Paderborn (3159)
GroupedDBID ---
-4V
-55
-5G
-BR
-EM
-Y2
-~C
-~X
.86
.VR
06D
0R~
0VY
123
199
1N0
1SB
2.D
203
28-
29P
2J2
2JN
2JY
2KG
2KM
2LR
2P1
2VQ
2~H
30V
36B
3V.
4.4
406
408
409
40D
40E
53G
5QI
5VS
67Z
6NX
78A
7WY
7X7
88E
8AO
8FI
8FJ
8FL
8G5
8TC
8UJ
8V8
95-
95.
95~
96X
AAAVM
AABHQ
AACDK
AAEOY
AAHNG
AAIAL
AAJBT
AAJKR
AANZL
AARHV
AARTL
AASML
AATNV
AATVU
AAUYE
AAWCG
AAYIU
AAYQN
AAYTO
ABAKF
ABBBX
ABBXA
ABDBF
ABDZT
ABECU
ABFGW
ABFTV
ABHLI
ABHQN
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABJOX
ABKAS
ABKCH
ABKTR
ABLJU
ABMNI
ABMQK
ABNWP
ABPLI
ABQBU
ABSXP
ABTEG
ABTHY
ABTKH
ABTMW
ABULA
ABUWG
ABWNU
ABXPI
ACAOD
ACBXY
ACDTI
ACGFS
ACHSB
ACHXU
ACIPQ
ACKIV
ACKNC
ACMDZ
ACMLO
ACNCT
ACOKC
ACOMO
ACPRK
ACSNA
ACWMK
ACYUM
ACZOJ
ADBBV
ADHHG
ADHIR
ADIMF
ADINQ
ADKNI
ADKPE
ADOXG
ADRFC
ADTPH
ADURQ
ADYFF
ADZKW
AEBTG
AEEQQ
AEFIE
AEFQL
AEGAL
AEGNC
AEJHL
AEJRE
AEKMD
AEMOZ
AEMSY
AEOHA
AEPYU
AESKC
AESTI
AETLH
AEVLU
AEVTX
AEXYK
AFEXP
AFFNX
AFGCZ
AFKRA
AFLOW
AFQWF
AFWTZ
AFZKB
AGAYW
AGDGC
AGGDS
AGJBK
AGMZJ
AGQEE
AGQMX
AGRTI
AGWIL
AGWZB
AGYKE
AHAVH
AHBYD
AHKAY
AHMBA
AHSBF
AHYZX
AIAKS
AIGIU
AIIXL
AILAN
AIMYW
AITGF
AJBLW
AJRNO
AJZVZ
AKQUC
AKVCP
ALIPV
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALWAN
AMKLP
AMXSW
AMYLF
AMYQR
AOCGG
AQQCX
ARMRJ
ASPBG
AVWKF
AXYYD
AYQZM
AZFZN
AZQEC
B-.
B0M
BA0
BBWZM
BDATZ
BENPR
BEZIV
BGNMA
BPHCQ
BVXVI
C6C
CAG
CCPQU
COF
CS3
CSCUP
DDRTE
DL5
DNIVK
DPUIP
DWQXO
EAD
EAP
EBA
EBC
EBD
EBLON
EBO
EBR
EBS
EBU
EIOEI
EJD
EMB
EMK
EMOBN
EPL
EPS
ESBYG
ESX
F5P
FEDTE
FERAY
FFXSO
FIGPU
FINBP
FNLPD
FRNLG
FRRFC
FSGXE
FWDCC
FYUFA
GGCAI
GGRSB
GJIRD
GNUQQ
GNWQR
GQ6
GQ7
GQ8
GROUPED_ABI_INFORM_COMPLETE
GUQSH
GXS
HF~
HG5
HG6
HMCUK
HMJXF
HQYDN
HRMNR
HVGLF
HZ~
H~9
I09
IHE
IJ-
IKXTQ
IRVIT
ITM
IWAJR
IXC
IZIGR
IZQ
I~X
I~Z
J-C
J0Z
JBSCW
JCJTX
JZLTJ
K1G
K60
K6~
KDC
KOV
KOW
LAS
LLZTM
LPU
M0C
M1P
M2M
M2O
M4Y
MA-
N2Q
N9A
NB0
NDZJH
NPVJJ
NQJWS
NU0
O-J
O9-
O93
O9G
O9I
O9J
OAM
P19
P9L
PF-
PQBIZ
PQBZA
PQQKQ
PROAC
PSQYO
PSYQQ
PT4
PT5
Q2X
QOK
QOS
R-Y
R89
R9I
RHV
RIG
RNI
ROL
RPX
RRX
RSV
RZK
S16
S1Z
S26
S27
S28
S3B
SAP
SBS
SBU
SCLPG
SDH
SDM
SHX
SISQX
SJYHP
SNE
SNPRN
SNX
SOHCF
SOJ
SPISZ
SRMVM
SSLCW
SSXJD
STPWE
SV3
SZN
T13
T16
TH9
TN5
TSG
TSK
TSV
TUC
TUS
U2A
U9L
UG4
UKHRP
UNUBA
UOJIU
UTJUX
UZXMN
VC2
VFIZW
W23
W48
WH7
WJK
WK6
WK8
YLTOR
YYP
Z45
Z83
Z88
Z8W
Z92
ZMTXR
ZMU
ZOVNA
ZXP
~8M
~EX
AAYZH
ADMHG
AFQWV
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
H13
NPM
AAYXX
CITATION
7TK
K9.
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c356t-1a0332cbf1fcbe91d4524a7367f2072baf17e3e88bd47c4f92a996528d498b5c3
IEDL.DBID AEJHL
ISSN 0340-0727
1430-2772
IngestDate Tue Sep 17 21:27:35 EDT 2024
Sat Oct 26 04:30:44 EDT 2024
Thu Oct 10 21:49:27 EDT 2024
Fri Nov 22 02:43:01 EST 2024
Sat Nov 02 12:26:00 EDT 2024
Sun Jul 28 01:20:49 EDT 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 5
Language English
License 2024. The Author(s).
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c356t-1a0332cbf1fcbe91d4524a7367f2072baf17e3e88bd47c4f92a996528d498b5c3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
ORCID 0000-0001-6256-8011
0000-0003-0549-5543
0000-0002-9350-5272
0000-0002-0348-9097
0000-0002-0236-7282
0000-0002-2395-4435
OpenAccessLink http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s00426-024-01977-2
PMID 38806734
PQID 3085004143
PQPubID 49159
PageCount 10
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_11281954
proquest_miscellaneous_3061784991
proquest_journals_3085004143
crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_024_01977_2
pubmed_primary_38806734
springer_journals_10_1007_s00426_024_01977_2
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2024-07-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2024-07-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 07
  year: 2024
  text: 2024-07-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace Berlin/Heidelberg
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Berlin/Heidelberg
– name: Germany
– name: Heidelberg
PublicationSubtitle An International Journal of Perception, Attention, Memory, and Action
PublicationTitle Psychological research
PublicationTitleAbbrev Psychological Research
PublicationTitleAlternate Psychol Res
PublicationYear 2024
Publisher Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Springer Nature B.V
Publisher_xml – name: Springer Berlin Heidelberg
– name: Springer Nature B.V
References KlauerKCMierkeJMuschJThe positivity proportion effect: A list-context effect in masked affective primingMemory and Cognition200331695396710.3758/BF0319644814651302
WendtMKieselAConflict adaptation in time: Foreperiods as contextual cues for attentional adjustmentPsychonomic Bulletin & Review20111891091610.3758/s13423-011-0119-4
CrumpMJCGongZMillikenBThe context-specific proportion congruent Stroop effect: Location as a contextual cuePsychonomic Bulletin & Review200613231632110.3758/BF03193850
GiesenCGSchmidtJRRothermundKThe law of recency: An episodic stimulus-response retrieval account of habit acquisitionFrontiers in Psychology2020102927292710.3389/fpsyg.2019.02927320100176974578
CrumpMJLearning to selectively attend from context-specific attentional histories: A demonstration and some constraintsCanadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale20167015910.1037/cep000006626372055
Rudolph, M., Giesen, C. G., & Rothermund, K. (2024). False contingency knowledge reverses the color-word contingency learning effect. [Manuscript submitted for publication].
Bugg, J. M. (2017). Context, conflict, and control. In T. Egner (Ed.), The Wiley Handbook of Cognitive Control (pp. 79–96). Wiley.
SchmidtJRGiesenCGRothermundKContingency learning as binding? Testing an exemplar view of the colour-word contingency learning effectQuarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology202073573976110.1177/1747021820906397
PolzienAGüldenpenningIWeigeltMA question of (perfect) timing: A preceding head turn increases the head-fake effect in basketballPlos One2021165e025111710.1371/journal.pone.0251117339793748115800
SchmidtJRCrumpMJCCheesmanJBesnerDContingency learning without awareness: Evidence for implicit controlConsciousness and Cognition200716242143510.1016/j.concog.2006.06.01016899377
Alhaj Ahmad AlaboudMGüldenpenningISteggemann-WeinrichYKundeWWeigeltMTäuschungshandlungen Im Sport: Der Blicktäuschungseffekt Im Basketball unter quasirealistischen BedingungenSportwissenschaft201646322323110.1007/s12662-016-0401-8
GüldenpenningIAlhaj Ahmad AlaboudMKundeWWeigeltMThe impact of global and local context information on the processing of deceptive actions in game sports: Both higher head-fake frequencies and head-fake repetitions reduce the size of the head-fake effect in basketballGerman Journal of Exercise and Sport Research201848336637510.1007/s12662-018-0493-4
SchmidtJRBesnerDThe Stroop effect: Why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingencyJournal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition200834351452318444752
KundeWSkirdeSWeigeltMTrust my face. Cognitive factors of head fakes in sportsJournal of Experimental Psychology: Applied201117211012721604910
BraemSBuggJMSchmidtJRCrumpMJCWeissmanDHNotebaertWEgnerTMeasuring adaptive control in conflict tasksTrends in Cognitive Science201923976978310.1016/j.tics.2019.07.002
ReussHDesenderKKieselAKundeWUnconscious conflicts in unconscious contexts: The role of awareness and timing in flexible conflict adaptationJournal of Experimental Psychology: General20141434170110.1037/a003643724708397
HutcheonTWhat is cued by faces in the face-based context-specific proportion congruent manipulation?Attention Perception & Psychophysics20228441248126310.3758/s13414-022-02447-w
GrattonGColesMGHDonchinEOptimizing the use of information. Strategic control of activation of responsesJournal of Experimental Psychology: General1992121448050610.1037/0096-3445.121.4.4801431740
SchmidtJRLemercierCContext-specific proportion congruent effects: Compound-cue contingency learning in disguiseQuarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology20197251119113010.1177/1747021818787155
BuggJMCrumpMJCIn support of a distinction between voluntary and stimulus-driven control: A review of the literature on proportion congruent effectsFrontiers in Psychology2012336710.3389/fpsyg.2012.00367230608363459019
HeinemannAKundeWKieselAContext-specific prime-congruency effects: On the role of conscious stimulus representations for cognitive controlConsciousness and Cognition200918496697610.1016/j.concog.2009.08.00919796967
Alhaj Ahmad Alaboud, M., Steggemann, Y., Klein-Soetebier, T., Kunde, W., & Weigelt, M. (2012). Täuschungshandlungen im Sport. Eine experimentelle Untersuchung Zur Wirkung der Häufigkeitsverteilung auf die Blicktäuschung im Basketball. Zeitschrift für Sportpsychologie, 19(3), 110–121.
GüldenpenningISchützCWeigeltMKundeWIs the head-fake effect in basketball robust against practice? Analyses of trial-by-trial adaptations, frequency distributions, and mixture effects to evaluate effects of practicePsychological Research Psychologische Forschung20208482383310.1007/s00426-018-1078-430128660
XuGMordkoffJTReliable correlational cuing while controlling for most-recent-pairing effectsFrontiers in Psychology20201159237710.3389/fpsyg.2020.592377333043017701050
JacobyLLLindsayDSHesselsSItem-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociationsPsychonomic Bulletin & Review200310363864410.3758/BF03196526
FringsCHommelBKochIRothermundKDignathDGiesenCKieselAKundeWMayrSMoellerBMöllerMPfisterRPhilippABinding and retrieval in action control (BRAC)Trends in Cognitive Sciences202024537538710.1016/j.tics.2020.02.00432298623
GüldenpenningIKundeWWeigeltMHead-fake perception in basketball: The relative contributions of expertise, visual or motor training, and test repetitionInternational Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology202020120222210.1080/1612197x.2020.1854819
HutcheonTGSpielerDHLimits on the generalizability of context-driven controlQuarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology20177071292130410.1080/17470218.2016.1182193
Rothermund, K., Gollnick, N., & Giesen, C. G. (2022). Accounting for proportion congruency effects in the Stroop task in a confounded setup: Retrieval of stimulus-response episodes explains it all. Journal of Cognition, 5(1): 39, pp. 1–16. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.232
SchouppeNRidderinkhofKRVergutsTNotebaertWContext-specific control and context selection in conflict tasksActa Psychologica2014146636610.1016/j.actpsy.2013.11.01024384400
BuggJMJacobyLLChananiSWhy it is too early to lose control in accounts of item-specific proportion congruency effectsJournal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance201137384485920718569
Campbell, J. I. D., & Thompson, V. A. (2012). MorePower 6.0 for ANOVA with relational confidence intervals and Bayesian analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 44(4), 1255–1265. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0186-0
WeigeltMGüldenpenningISteggemann-WeinrichYAlhaj Ahmad AlaboudMKundeWControl over the processing of the opponent’s gaze direction in basketball expertsPsychonomic Bulletin & Review201724382883410.3758/s13423-016-1140-4
ArunkumarMRothermundKKundeWGiesenCGBeing in the know: The role of awareness and retrieval of transient stimulus-response bindings in selective contingency learningJournal of Cognition2022513610.5334/joc.227360720919400625
BuggJMHutchisonKAConverging evidence for control of color–word Stroop interference at the item levelJournal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance201339243344922845037
CañadasERodríguez-BailónRMillikenBLupiáñezJSocial categories as a context for the allocation of attentional controlJournal of Experimental Psychology: General2013142393494310.1037/a002979422905873
LoganGDZbrodoffNJWhen it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like taskMemory & Cognition19797316617410.3758/BF03197535
LehleCHübnerROn-the-fly adaptation of selectivity in the flanker taskPsychonomic Bulletin & Review20081581481810.3758/PBR.15.4.814
RudolphMRothermundKTwo sources of color-word contingency learning: Episodic retrieval of SR bindings and propositional knowledgeJournal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition202410.1037/xlm000135338815114
Giesen, C. G., Duderstadt, H., Richter, J., & Rothermund, K. (2024). Dissociating the roles of episodic response retrieval and contingency awareness in valence contingency learning. [Manuscript submitted for publication].
LoweDGMittererJOSelective and divided attention in a Stroop taskCanadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie198236468470010.1037/h0080661
1977_CR34
LL Jacoby (1977_CR23) 2003; 10
1977_CR32
1977_CR5
JR Schmidt (1977_CR39) 2020; 73
1977_CR2
M Arunkumar (1977_CR3) 2022; 5
I Güldenpenning (1977_CR18) 2020; 20
G Gratton (1977_CR16) 1992; 121
JM Bugg (1977_CR6) 2012; 3
A Heinemann (1977_CR20) 2009; 18
1977_CR14
MJ Crump (1977_CR10) 2016; 70
M Weigelt (1977_CR41) 2017; 24
JM Bugg (1977_CR8) 2011; 37
T Hutcheon (1977_CR21) 2022; 84
JR Schmidt (1977_CR38) 2019; 72
M Wendt (1977_CR43) 2011; 18
C Frings (1977_CR13) 2020; 24
C Lehle (1977_CR26) 2008; 15
GD Logan (1977_CR27) 1979; 7
JM Bugg (1977_CR7) 2013; 39
I Güldenpenning (1977_CR17) 2018; 48
1977_CR45
N Schouppe (1977_CR40) 2014; 146
JR Schmidt (1977_CR37) 2007; 16
S Braem (1977_CR4) 2019; 23
M Alhaj Ahmad Alaboud (1977_CR1) 2016; 46
MJC Crump (1977_CR11) 2006; 13
I Güldenpenning (1977_CR19) 2020; 84
CG Giesen (1977_CR15) 2020; 10
W Kunde (1977_CR25) 2011; 17
DG Lowe (1977_CR28) 1982; 36
H Reuss (1977_CR31) 2014; 143
KC Klauer (1977_CR24) 2003; 31
E Cañadas (1977_CR9) 2013; 142
JR Schmidt (1977_CR36) 2008; 34
G Xu (1977_CR44) 2020; 11
A Polzien (1977_CR29) 2021; 16
TG Hutcheon (1977_CR22) 2017; 70
M Rudolph (1977_CR35) 2024
References_xml – volume: 18
  start-page: 966
  issue: 4
  year: 2009
  ident: 1977_CR20
  publication-title: Consciousness and Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.08.009
  contributor:
    fullname: A Heinemann
– volume: 18
  start-page: 910
  year: 2011
  ident: 1977_CR43
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0119-4
  contributor:
    fullname: M Wendt
– year: 2024
  ident: 1977_CR35
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition
  doi: 10.1037/xlm0001353
  contributor:
    fullname: M Rudolph
– volume: 72
  start-page: 1119
  issue: 5
  year: 2019
  ident: 1977_CR38
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1177/1747021818787155
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– ident: 1977_CR5
  doi: 10.1002/9781118920497.ch5
– volume: 34
  start-page: 514
  issue: 3
  year: 2008
  ident: 1977_CR36
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– volume: 13
  start-page: 316
  issue: 2
  year: 2006
  ident: 1977_CR11
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03193850
  contributor:
    fullname: MJC Crump
– ident: 1977_CR2
  doi: 10.1026/1612-5010/a000075
– volume: 24
  start-page: 375
  issue: 5
  year: 2020
  ident: 1977_CR13
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.02.004
  contributor:
    fullname: C Frings
– volume: 11
  start-page: 592377
  year: 2020
  ident: 1977_CR44
  publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.592377
  contributor:
    fullname: G Xu
– volume: 70
  start-page: 59
  issue: 1
  year: 2016
  ident: 1977_CR10
  publication-title: Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale
  doi: 10.1037/cep0000066
  contributor:
    fullname: MJ Crump
– ident: 1977_CR14
– volume: 15
  start-page: 814
  year: 2008
  ident: 1977_CR26
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.4.814
  contributor:
    fullname: C Lehle
– volume: 73
  start-page: 739
  issue: 5
  year: 2020
  ident: 1977_CR39
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1177/1747021820906397
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– volume: 20
  start-page: 202
  issue: 1
  year: 2020
  ident: 1977_CR18
  publication-title: International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology
  doi: 10.1080/1612197x.2020.1854819
  contributor:
    fullname: I Güldenpenning
– volume: 16
  start-page: e0251117
  issue: 5
  year: 2021
  ident: 1977_CR29
  publication-title: Plos One
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0251117
  contributor:
    fullname: A Polzien
– volume: 37
  start-page: 844
  issue: 3
  year: 2011
  ident: 1977_CR8
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  contributor:
    fullname: JM Bugg
– volume: 16
  start-page: 421
  issue: 2
  year: 2007
  ident: 1977_CR37
  publication-title: Consciousness and Cognition
  doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2006.06.010
  contributor:
    fullname: JR Schmidt
– volume: 5
  start-page: 36
  issue: 1
  year: 2022
  ident: 1977_CR3
  publication-title: Journal of Cognition
  doi: 10.5334/joc.227
  contributor:
    fullname: M Arunkumar
– volume: 23
  start-page: 769
  issue: 9
  year: 2019
  ident: 1977_CR4
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Science
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.07.002
  contributor:
    fullname: S Braem
– volume: 39
  start-page: 433
  issue: 2
  year: 2013
  ident: 1977_CR7
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
  contributor:
    fullname: JM Bugg
– volume: 142
  start-page: 934
  issue: 3
  year: 2013
  ident: 1977_CR9
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/a0029794
  contributor:
    fullname: E Cañadas
– volume: 17
  start-page: 110
  issue: 2
  year: 2011
  ident: 1977_CR25
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied
  contributor:
    fullname: W Kunde
– volume: 10
  start-page: 638
  issue: 3
  year: 2003
  ident: 1977_CR23
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/BF03196526
  contributor:
    fullname: LL Jacoby
– volume: 146
  start-page: 63
  year: 2014
  ident: 1977_CR40
  publication-title: Acta Psychologica
  doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.11.010
  contributor:
    fullname: N Schouppe
– ident: 1977_CR45
  doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0186-0
– volume: 10
  start-page: 2927
  issue: 2927
  year: 2020
  ident: 1977_CR15
  publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02927
  contributor:
    fullname: CG Giesen
– volume: 3
  start-page: 367
  year: 2012
  ident: 1977_CR6
  publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00367
  contributor:
    fullname: JM Bugg
– volume: 121
  start-page: 480
  issue: 4
  year: 1992
  ident: 1977_CR16
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.480
  contributor:
    fullname: G Gratton
– volume: 84
  start-page: 823
  year: 2020
  ident: 1977_CR19
  publication-title: Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung
  doi: 10.1007/s00426-018-1078-4
  contributor:
    fullname: I Güldenpenning
– volume: 24
  start-page: 828
  issue: 3
  year: 2017
  ident: 1977_CR41
  publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1140-4
  contributor:
    fullname: M Weigelt
– volume: 70
  start-page: 1292
  issue: 7
  year: 2017
  ident: 1977_CR22
  publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
  doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1182193
  contributor:
    fullname: TG Hutcheon
– volume: 84
  start-page: 1248
  issue: 4
  year: 2022
  ident: 1977_CR21
  publication-title: Attention Perception & Psychophysics
  doi: 10.3758/s13414-022-02447-w
  contributor:
    fullname: T Hutcheon
– volume: 143
  start-page: 1701
  issue: 4
  year: 2014
  ident: 1977_CR31
  publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
  doi: 10.1037/a0036437
  contributor:
    fullname: H Reuss
– volume: 46
  start-page: 223
  issue: 3
  year: 2016
  ident: 1977_CR1
  publication-title: Sportwissenschaft
  doi: 10.1007/s12662-016-0401-8
  contributor:
    fullname: M Alhaj Ahmad Alaboud
– volume: 31
  start-page: 953
  issue: 6
  year: 2003
  ident: 1977_CR24
  publication-title: Memory and Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03196448
  contributor:
    fullname: KC Klauer
– volume: 36
  start-page: 684
  issue: 4
  year: 1982
  ident: 1977_CR28
  publication-title: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie
  doi: 10.1037/h0080661
  contributor:
    fullname: DG Lowe
– ident: 1977_CR32
  doi: 10.5334/joc.232
– volume: 7
  start-page: 166
  issue: 3
  year: 1979
  ident: 1977_CR27
  publication-title: Memory & Cognition
  doi: 10.3758/BF03197535
  contributor:
    fullname: GD Logan
– ident: 1977_CR34
– volume: 48
  start-page: 366
  issue: 3
  year: 2018
  ident: 1977_CR17
  publication-title: German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research
  doi: 10.1007/s12662-018-0493-4
  contributor:
    fullname: I Güldenpenning
SSID ssj0017667
Score 2.4289403
Snippet In basketball, an attacking player often plays a pass to one side while looking to the other side. This head fake provokes a conflict in the observing...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
crossref
pubmed
springer
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Publisher
StartPage 1702
SubjectTerms Adaptation
Adaptation, Psychological - physiology
Adult
Athletic Performance - physiology
Basketball
Basketball - physiology
Behavioral Science and Psychology
Deception
Female
Head - physiology
Humans
Information processing
Male
Memory
Psychology
Psychology Research
Psychomotor Performance - physiology
Reaction Time - physiology
Young Adult
Title Context-specific adaptation for head fakes in basketball: a study on player-specific fake-frequency schedules
URI https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s00426-024-01977-2
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38806734
https://www.proquest.com/docview/3085004143
https://www.proquest.com/docview/3061784991
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC11281954
Volume 88
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://sdu.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV07T8MwED5BWVh4P8JLRmIDoyR2EocNQUtBiAWQ2CLbcUTVEiraDvx7zk4aVB4DTBlyefjO9n2Xu_sCcJQrP06FsFldX1Ee85QKoXyqfS5zGRY-M7YbuXuf3D2Jy7alyWHNp4uyfzrNSLqNuul1c2ifokvB6BdBC8V9dwF9T8RbsHDevuneNsmDJHY_jvWZrVxE_1z3yvx8l1l_9A1kfq-V_JIwdX6os_yvEazAUg07yXk1T1ZhzpRrsNjsfu_r8OJ4qjAMtr2Xtn6IoBaHVaKeILIluGvnpJB9MyK9kqD365uxkoPBGZHEkdQSFBwOJGL4z3tYeVq8VfXa7wRDaXRtAzPagMdO--GiS-t_MVDNonhMA-kzFmpVBIVWJg1yHoVcJixOihB1rGQRJIYZtHPOE82LNJQYSUWhyHkqVKTZJrTK19JsA1EiilUUaMm5xMmhUo2gMRCxRuyB0VPkwfHUItmwotzIGnJlp8MMdZg5HWahB3tTo2X18htlzBLx-RyxoAeHzWlcODYbIkvzOrEytjsSA77Ag63Kxs3jLENOnDDugZixfiNgSblnz5S9Z0fOHVSpSbz0ZDoLPt_r92Hs_E18FxZDN5Fs3fAetMZvE7MP86N8clCvBnu8frjqfABx3wWq
link.rule.ids 230,315,782,786,887,27934,27935,41074,42143,48345,48348,49650,49653,52154
linkProvider Springer Nature
linkToHtml http://sdu.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1LT9wwEB6V5VAuQCmP8GhdiRtYcmIncbghYFkE5cJW6i2yHUcglrBidw_8e8bOA20pBzh74iQztucbzcxngP1CsyST0mV1maYiERmVUjNqmFCFikrGretGHtyk13_l6ZmjyRFtL4yvdm9Tkv6k7prdPNyn6FMw_EXUQvHgXXRs56wHi8cXw_N-lz1IE39zLOOudBEddNMs8_9Z5h3SG5T5tljyn4ypd0T9lc_9wiosN8CTHNcr5Rt8sdUaLHXn3_N3ePBMVRgIu-5LV0FEUI_jOlVPENsSPLcLUqp7OyF3FUH_d2-nWo1GR0QRT1NLUHA8UojiX-dw8rR8qiu2nwkG0-jcRnayDn_6Z8OTAW1uY6CGx8mUhopxHhldhqXRNgsLEUdCpTxJywiVrFUZppZbtHQhUiPKLFIYS8WRLEQmdWz4BvSqx8puAdEyTnQcGiWEwuWhM4OwMZSJQfSB8VMcwEFrknxck27kHb2y12GOOsy9DvMogN3WanmzASc5d1R8TCAaDOBXN4xbx-VDVGUfZ07G9UdiyBcGsFkbuXud48hJUi4CkHPm7wQcLff8SHV36-m5wzo5iY8etqvg9bve_43tj4n_hK-D4e-r_Ori-nIHliK_qFwV8S70pk8zuwcLk2L2o9kaL8aBCAM
linkToPdf http://sdu.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV1LT-MwEB6xRUJceO4u4bVG4sZaJLGTONwQtCoLQkiAtLfIdmwtooSKtgf-PWMnDSqwB8TZEycZjz3faGY-A-yXKkxzIVxWN1SUpzynQqiQ6pDLUsY2ZMZ1I_evs8u_4rTraHLaLn5f7T5NSdY9DY6lqRofDkt72Da-eehP0b9gKIwIhuIhPM9dbWEH5o-7f_oXbSYhS_0tsiFzZYzorJvGmY9nmXVO7xDn-8LJN9lT75R6y1__nRVYagApOa4taBXmTLUGi-25-LwOD57BCgNk15XpKosI6ndYp_AJYl6C53lJrLw3I3JXEfSL92as5GBwRCTx9LUEBYcDiej-dQ4nT-1TXcn9TDDIRqc3MKPvcNvr3pz0aXNLA9UsScc0kiFjsVY2slqZPCp5EnOZsTSzMSpcSRtlhhm0gJJnmts8lhhjJbEoeS5UotkP6FSPldkAokSSqiTSknOJZqNyjXAyEqlGVIJxVRLAwXR5imFNxlG0tMtehwXqsPA6LOIAtqcrWDQbc1QwR9EXckSJAey1w7ilXJ5EVuZx4mRc3ySGglEAP-sFb1_nuHPSjPEAxIwptAKOrnt2pLr752m7ozppiY_-nlrE63f9_zc2Pyf-CxauTnvFxdnl-RYsxt6mXHHxNnTGTxOzA99G5WS32SUv9UcQzw
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Context-specific+adaptation+for+head+fakes+in+basketball%3A+a+study+on+player-specific+fake-frequency+schedules&rft.jtitle=Psychological+research&rft.au=G%C3%BCldenpenning%2C+Iris&rft.au=B%C3%B6er%2C+Nils+T.&rft.au=Kunde%2C+Wilfried&rft.au=Giesen%2C+Carina+G.&rft.date=2024-07-01&rft.pub=Springer+Berlin+Heidelberg&rft.issn=0340-0727&rft.eissn=1430-2772&rft.volume=88&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=1702&rft.epage=1711&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007%2Fs00426-024-01977-2&rft.externalDocID=10_1007_s00426_024_01977_2
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0340-0727&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0340-0727&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0340-0727&client=summon