Mechanisms of spatial contextual cueing in younger and older adults

The contextual cueing effect is the phenomenon observed when response time (RT) becomes faster in visual search in repeated context compared with a new one. In the present study, we explored whether the mechanisms involved in the effect are age dependent. We investigated it in younger (N = 20, 12 wo...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published in:Psychophysiology Vol. 60; no. 11; p. e14361
Main Authors: Kojouharova, Petia, Nagy, Boglárka, Czigler, István, Gaál, Zsófia Anna
Format: Journal Article
Language:English
Published: United States Blackwell Publishing Ltd 01-11-2023
Subjects:
Online Access:Get full text
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
Abstract The contextual cueing effect is the phenomenon observed when response time (RT) becomes faster in visual search in repeated context compared with a new one. In the present study, we explored whether the mechanisms involved in the effect are age dependent. We investigated it in younger (N = 20, 12 women, 21.2 ± 1.75 years) and older (N = 19, nine women, 67.05 ± 3.94 years) adults. We found a faster target identification in the repeated configurations with similar magnitude in the two age groups, which indicates that this contextual cueing effect remained intact even in the older participants. To shed light on the underlying mechanisms, we measured and compared the amplitude of three event-related potentials: N2pc, P3, and response-locked LRP. In the younger group, the larger contextual cueing effect (novel-minus-repeated RT difference) correlated positively with a larger difference in amplitude for repeated compared with novel configurations for both the N2pc and the P3 components, but there was no correlation with the response-locked lateralized readiness potential (rLRP) amplitude difference. However, in the older group, only the rLRP amplitude difference between novel and repeated configurations showed an enhancement with larger contextual cueing. These results suggest that different mechanisms are responsible for the contextual effect in the two age groups. It has both an early and an intermediate locus in younger adults: effective attentional allocation and successful stimulus categorization, or decision-making confidence are involved; while in older adults, a late locus was identified: a more efficient response organization led to a faster reaction.
AbstractList The contextual cueing effect is the phenomenon observed when response time (RT) becomes faster in visual search in repeated context compared with a new one. In the present study, we explored whether the mechanisms involved in the effect are age dependent. We investigated it in younger (N = 20, 12 women, 21.2 ± 1.75 years) and older (N = 19, nine women, 67.05 ± 3.94 years) adults. We found a faster target identification in the repeated configurations with similar magnitude in the two age groups, which indicates that this contextual cueing effect remained intact even in the older participants. To shed light on the underlying mechanisms, we measured and compared the amplitude of three event‐related potentials: N2pc, P3, and response‐locked LRP. In the younger group, the larger contextual cueing effect (novel‐minus‐repeated RT difference) correlated positively with a larger difference in amplitude for repeated compared with novel configurations for both the N2pc and the P3 components, but there was no correlation with the response‐locked lateralized readiness potential (rLRP) amplitude difference. However, in the older group, only the rLRP amplitude difference between novel and repeated configurations showed an enhancement with larger contextual cueing. These results suggest that different mechanisms are responsible for the contextual effect in the two age groups. It has both an early and an intermediate locus in younger adults: effective attentional allocation and successful stimulus categorization, or decision‐making confidence are involved; while in older adults, a late locus was identified: a more efficient response organization led to a faster reaction.
The contextual cueing effect is the phenomenon observed when response time (RT) becomes faster in visual search in repeated context compared with a new one. In the present study, we explored whether the mechanisms involved in the effect are age dependent. We investigated it in younger ( N  = 20, 12 women, 21.2 ± 1.75 years) and older ( N  = 19, nine women, 67.05 ± 3.94 years) adults. We found a faster target identification in the repeated configurations with similar magnitude in the two age groups, which indicates that this contextual cueing effect remained intact even in the older participants. To shed light on the underlying mechanisms, we measured and compared the amplitude of three event‐related potentials: N2pc, P3, and response‐locked LRP. In the younger group, the larger contextual cueing effect (novel‐minus‐repeated RT difference) correlated positively with a larger difference in amplitude for repeated compared with novel configurations for both the N2pc and the P3 components, but there was no correlation with the response‐locked lateralized readiness potential (rLRP) amplitude difference. However, in the older group, only the rLRP amplitude difference between novel and repeated configurations showed an enhancement with larger contextual cueing. These results suggest that different mechanisms are responsible for the contextual effect in the two age groups. It has both an early and an intermediate locus in younger adults: effective attentional allocation and successful stimulus categorization, or decision‐making confidence are involved; while in older adults, a late locus was identified: a more efficient response organization led to a faster reaction. In our study, we show that younger and older adults rely on different mechanisms when using contextual information to improve performance in a visual search task. Finding a target in repeated spatial configurations compared with new ones (contextual cueing effect) was equally faster in both age groups, which was a consequence of more efficient attention allocation and stimuli identification in younger and more efficient inhibition of distractor stimuli and response organization in older adults.
Author Kojouharova, Petia
Nagy, Boglárka
Czigler, István
Gaál, Zsófia Anna
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Petia
  orcidid: 0000-0001-6315-4822
  surname: Kojouharova
  fullname: Kojouharova, Petia
  organization: Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Budapest, Hungary
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Boglárka
  orcidid: 0000-0001-7897-839X
  surname: Nagy
  fullname: Nagy, Boglárka
  organization: Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Budapest, Hungary
– sequence: 3
  givenname: István
  orcidid: 0000-0003-1661-3808
  surname: Czigler
  fullname: Czigler, István
  organization: Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Budapest, Hungary
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Zsófia Anna
  orcidid: 0000-0002-6615-5393
  surname: Gaál
  fullname: Gaál, Zsófia Anna
  organization: Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Budapest, Hungary
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37294010$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNpd0MtKxDAUgOEgI85FNz6AFNyI0DG35rKUwRuMuNF1SdNk7NBJatOA8_amjrowm3MWH4fwz8HEeWcAOEdwidK76cK-WyJKGDoCM1RQmBeyEBMwg5CKvOAcT8E8hC2EUCKMT8CUcCwpRHAGVs9GvyvXhF3IvM1Cp4ZGtZn2bjCfQxzXaBq3yRqX7X10G9NnytWZb-txq2M7hFNwbFUbzNnPXIC3-7vX1WO-fnl4Wt2uc00KNOQ1rQWRTNvKQqOgqgSTigioJKeisMgyCSvDuMWaaSGNNZgVpkqmIkhySRbg6nC36_1HNGEod03Qpm2VMz6GEgtMmaCcwEQv_9Gtj71Lv0sqBWGCQ5zU9UHp3ofQG1t2fbNT_b5EsBzTlmPa8jttwhc_J2O1M_Uf_W1JvgCw9XXF
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhae175
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_02135
crossref_primary_10_3389_fcogn_2024_1336379
crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218241241954
Cites_doi 10.1037/xhp0000394
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.022
10.1080/13506285.2019.1668516
10.3758/s13415‐016‐0442‐3
10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2007.00611.x
10.1111/j.1469‐8986.1977.tb01312.x
10.1093/geronb/62.2.P78
10.1126/science.172.3990.1357
10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2009.00807.x
10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.12.012
10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.011
10.1006/cogp.1998.0681
10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.021
10.1111/psyp.13542
10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
10.1017/S0140525X00058027
10.3758/BF03193146
10.1093/brain/awh042
10.1016/j.clinph.2013.05.012
10.1037/cep0000007
10.3758/BF03206196
10.1027//0269‐8803.16.3.167
10.3389/fnhum.2020.569614
10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.02.007
10.1016/S0168‐5597(97)96139‐6
10.1037/0882‐7974.17.1.85
10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.06.004
10.1016/0168‐5597(88)90019‐6
10.3758/s13414‐019‐01832‐2
10.1093/cercor/bhm155
10.1016/0013‐4694(96)95711‐9
10.1111/j.1469‐8986.1980.tb00131.x
10.1016/j.brainres.2008.03.025
10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2005.00285.x
10.1037/0894‐4105.18.1.124
10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.009
10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.02.025
10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.09.011
10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.003
10.3758/BF03194108
10.3758/BF03212388
10.1111/psyp.12063
10.1037/h0020586
10.3389/fnagi.2020.596047
10.1007/s11065‐014‐9270‐9
10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.08.003
10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2007.00602.x
10.1037/0033‐295X.103.3.403
10.1037/0096‐1523.33.4.816
10.1037/0096‐1523.1.3.268
10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213
10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2010.00979.x
10.1007/s00426‐008‐0211‐1
10.1371/journal.pone.0206468
10.1016/0168‐5597(94)00256‐E
10.1037/a0022014
10.1007/978-1-4684-4178-9_11
10.1007/s11065‐009‐9117‐y
10.1037/0096‐1523.20.5.1000
10.1111/j.1467‐8721.2007.00478.x
10.1017/S0140525X00058015
10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.378
10.1111/ejn.12936
10.1177/0956797620954815
10.1111/j.1469‐8986.1986.tb00649.x
10.1016/j.brainres.2015.07.032
10.1080/713756001
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2023 The Authors. Psychophysiology published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of Society for Psychophysiological Research.
2023. This article is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Copyright_xml – notice: 2023 The Authors. Psychophysiology published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of Society for Psychophysiological Research.
– notice: 2023. This article is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
DBID NPM
AAYXX
CITATION
7TK
K9.
7X8
DOI 10.1111/psyp.14361
DatabaseName PubMed
CrossRef
Neurosciences Abstracts
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitle PubMed
CrossRef
ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)
PubMed
CrossRef
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Anatomy & Physiology
EISSN 1540-5958
1469-8986
EndPage e14361
ExternalDocumentID 10_1111_psyp_14361
37294010
Genre Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Hungarian Scientific Research Fund
  grantid: OTKA K 132880
GroupedDBID ---
--Z
-DZ
-~X
.3N
.GA
05W
0R~
10A
123
1OB
1OC
29P
2QV
33P
36B
4.4
50Y
50Z
51W
51Y
52M
52O
52Q
52R
52S
52T
52U
52V
52W
5HH
5LA
5RE
5VS
66C
702
7PT
8-0
8-1
8-3
8-4
8-5
8UM
930
A01
A04
AABNI
AAESR
AAHHS
AAONW
AAOUF
AAXRX
AAZKR
ABCQN
ABCUV
ABDBF
ABGDZ
ABITZ
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABLJU
ABPPZ
ABPVW
ABQWH
ABSOO
ABXGK
ACAHQ
ACBKW
ACCFJ
ACCZN
ACFBH
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACGOF
ACHQT
ACMXC
ACNCT
ACPOU
ACPRK
ACWUS
ACXQS
ADBBV
ADBTR
ADEMA
ADEOM
ADIZJ
ADKYN
ADMGS
ADXAS
ADZMN
AEEZP
AEGXH
AEIGN
AEIMD
AENEX
AEQDE
AEUQT
AEUYR
AFBPY
AFEBI
AFFPM
AFGKR
AFKFF
AFPWT
AFZJQ
AHBTC
AHMBA
AIACR
AIAGR
AIFKG
AIURR
AIWBW
AJBDE
ALAGY
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALUQN
AMBMR
AMYDB
ASTYK
AZBYB
AZVAB
BAFTC
BDRZF
BFHJK
BMXJE
BNVMJ
BQESF
BROTX
BRXPI
BY8
CS3
D-6
D-7
D-C
D-D
DCZOG
DPXWK
DR2
DRFUL
DRMAN
DRSSH
DU5
DXH
EAD
EAP
EBC
EBD
EBS
EMB
EMK
EMOBN
EPS
ESX
F00
F01
F5P
FUBAC
G-S
G.N
G50
GODZA
HGLYW
HZI
HZ~
IHE
IX1
J0M
K48
KBYEO
L7B
LATKE
LC2
LC4
LEEKS
LH4
LITHE
LOXES
LP6
LP7
LUTES
LYRES
MEWTI
MK4
MRFUL
MRMAN
MRSSH
MSFUL
MSMAN
MSSSH
MXFUL
MXMAN
MXSSH
N04
N06
N9A
NF~
NPM
O66
O9-
OIG
P2W
P2Y
P2Z
P4B
P4C
PQQKQ
Q.N
Q11
QB0
R.K
RCA
RIG
ROL
RX1
S10
SUPJJ
SV3
TN5
TUS
UB1
V8K
VQA
W8V
W99
WBKPD
WH7
WHDPE
WIH
WII
WIJ
WOHZO
WQZ
WRC
WSUWO
WXI
WXSBR
XG1
XSW
YNT
YYM
YYP
YZZ
ZCA
ZZTAW
~IA
~WP
.55
.GJ
.Y3
186
31~
53G
AAMNL
AASGY
AAYJJ
AAYXX
ABDPE
ABEML
ACBWZ
ACQPF
ACSCC
AFFNX
AHEFC
ASPBG
AVWKF
AZFZN
CAG
CITATION
COF
DC6
EJD
FEDTE
FZ0
HAOEW
HF~
HVGLF
LPU
LW6
MVM
NEJ
OHT
OVD
PALCI
RIWAO
RJQFR
SAMSI
TEORI
UAP
X7M
XJT
XOL
ZGI
ZWS
ZXP
7TK
K9.
7X8
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c351t-d4d8396cfbf0ea0ab869a380a97485f1f690be67f2c6c89efe265eb9a3b319793
ISSN 0048-5772
IngestDate Sat Oct 05 05:41:53 EDT 2024
Thu Oct 10 15:51:12 EDT 2024
Thu Nov 21 23:41:33 EST 2024
Sat Sep 28 08:11:41 EDT 2024
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 11
Keywords ERP
contextual cueing
N2pc
P3
LRP
aging
Language English
License 2023 The Authors. Psychophysiology published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of Society for Psychophysiological Research.
LinkModel OpenURL
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c351t-d4d8396cfbf0ea0ab869a380a97485f1f690be67f2c6c89efe265eb9a3b319793
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
ORCID 0000-0003-1661-3808
0000-0002-6615-5393
0000-0001-6315-4822
0000-0001-7897-839X
OpenAccessLink https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdfdirect/10.1111/psyp.14361
PMID 37294010
PQID 2877268702
PQPubID 41359
ParticipantIDs proquest_miscellaneous_2824684730
proquest_journals_2877268702
crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_14361
pubmed_primary_37294010
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2023-11-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2023-11-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 11
  year: 2023
  text: 2023-11-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2020
PublicationPlace United States
PublicationPlace_xml – name: United States
– name: Oxford
PublicationTitle Psychophysiology
PublicationTitleAlternate Psychophysiology
PublicationYear 2023
Publisher Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Publisher_xml – name: Blackwell Publishing Ltd
References e_1_2_9_31_1
e_1_2_9_52_1
e_1_2_9_50_1
e_1_2_9_73_1
e_1_2_9_10_1
e_1_2_9_35_1
e_1_2_9_56_1
e_1_2_9_12_1
e_1_2_9_33_1
e_1_2_9_71_1
e_1_2_9_14_1
e_1_2_9_39_1
e_1_2_9_16_1
e_1_2_9_37_1
e_1_2_9_58_1
e_1_2_9_18_1
e_1_2_9_41_1
e_1_2_9_64_1
e_1_2_9_20_1
e_1_2_9_62_1
e_1_2_9_22_1
e_1_2_9_45_1
e_1_2_9_68_1
e_1_2_9_24_1
e_1_2_9_43_1
e_1_2_9_66_1
e_1_2_9_8_1
Luck S. J. (e_1_2_9_36_1) 1990; 16
e_1_2_9_6_1
e_1_2_9_4_1
e_1_2_9_60_1
e_1_2_9_2_1
e_1_2_9_26_1
e_1_2_9_49_1
e_1_2_9_28_1
e_1_2_9_47_1
e_1_2_9_30_1
e_1_2_9_53_1
e_1_2_9_74_1
e_1_2_9_51_1
e_1_2_9_72_1
e_1_2_9_11_1
e_1_2_9_34_1
e_1_2_9_57_1
e_1_2_9_13_1
e_1_2_9_32_1
Cogent 2000 Team (e_1_2_9_7_1) 2003
e_1_2_9_70_1
e_1_2_9_15_1
e_1_2_9_38_1
Rózsa S. (e_1_2_9_55_1) 2010
e_1_2_9_17_1
e_1_2_9_59_1
e_1_2_9_19_1
e_1_2_9_42_1
e_1_2_9_63_1
e_1_2_9_40_1
e_1_2_9_61_1
e_1_2_9_21_1
e_1_2_9_46_1
e_1_2_9_67_1
e_1_2_9_23_1
e_1_2_9_44_1
e_1_2_9_65_1
e_1_2_9_5_1
e_1_2_9_3_1
Romaya J. (e_1_2_9_54_1) 2008
e_1_2_9_9_1
e_1_2_9_25_1
e_1_2_9_27_1
e_1_2_9_48_1
e_1_2_9_69_1
e_1_2_9_29_1
References_xml – ident: e_1_2_9_20_1
  doi: 10.1037/xhp0000394
– ident: e_1_2_9_66_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.022
– ident: e_1_2_9_49_1
  doi: 10.1080/13506285.2019.1668516
– ident: e_1_2_9_31_1
  doi: 10.3758/s13415‐016‐0442‐3
– ident: e_1_2_9_29_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2007.00611.x
– ident: e_1_2_9_12_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.1977.tb01312.x
– ident: e_1_2_9_53_1
  doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.2.P78
– volume-title: Cogent graphics [Computer software]
  year: 2008
  ident: e_1_2_9_54_1
  contributor:
    fullname: Romaya J.
– ident: e_1_2_9_21_1
  doi: 10.1126/science.172.3990.1357
– ident: e_1_2_9_58_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2009.00807.x
– ident: e_1_2_9_48_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.12.012
– ident: e_1_2_9_72_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.011
– ident: e_1_2_9_6_1
  doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0681
– ident: e_1_2_9_44_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.021
– ident: e_1_2_9_68_1
  doi: 10.1111/psyp.13542
– ident: e_1_2_9_10_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
– ident: e_1_2_9_11_1
  doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00058027
– ident: e_1_2_9_16_1
  doi: 10.3758/BF03193146
– ident: e_1_2_9_73_1
  doi: 10.1093/brain/awh042
– ident: e_1_2_9_42_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.05.012
– ident: e_1_2_9_38_1
  doi: 10.1037/cep0000007
– ident: e_1_2_9_45_1
  doi: 10.3758/BF03206196
– ident: e_1_2_9_15_1
  doi: 10.1027//0269‐8803.16.3.167
– ident: e_1_2_9_18_1
  doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.569614
– ident: e_1_2_9_35_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.02.007
– ident: e_1_2_9_47_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0168‐5597(97)96139‐6
– ident: e_1_2_9_3_1
  doi: 10.1037/0882‐7974.17.1.85
– ident: e_1_2_9_41_1
– ident: e_1_2_9_2_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.06.004
– ident: e_1_2_9_46_1
  doi: 10.1016/0168‐5597(88)90019‐6
– ident: e_1_2_9_61_1
  doi: 10.3758/s13414‐019‐01832‐2
– ident: e_1_2_9_9_1
  doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm155
– ident: e_1_2_9_13_1
  doi: 10.1016/0013‐4694(96)95711‐9
– ident: e_1_2_9_27_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.1980.tb00131.x
– ident: e_1_2_9_71_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.03.025
– ident: e_1_2_9_14_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2005.00285.x
– ident: e_1_2_9_22_1
  doi: 10.1037/0894‐4105.18.1.124
– ident: e_1_2_9_19_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.009
– ident: e_1_2_9_33_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.02.025
– ident: e_1_2_9_70_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.09.011
– ident: e_1_2_9_57_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.003
– ident: e_1_2_9_25_1
  doi: 10.3758/BF03194108
– ident: e_1_2_9_63_1
  doi: 10.3758/BF03212388
– ident: e_1_2_9_4_1
  doi: 10.1111/psyp.12063
– ident: e_1_2_9_60_1
  doi: 10.1037/h0020586
– ident: e_1_2_9_30_1
  doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.596047
– ident: e_1_2_9_51_1
  doi: 10.1007/s11065‐014‐9270‐9
– volume: 16
  start-page: 577
  year: 1990
  ident: e_1_2_9_36_1
  article-title: Evidence for an ERP correlate of attentional filtering
  publication-title: Society for Neuroscience Abstracts
  contributor:
    fullname: Luck S. J.
– ident: e_1_2_9_28_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.08.003
– volume-title: WAIS‐IV. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition
  year: 2010
  ident: e_1_2_9_55_1
  contributor:
    fullname: Rózsa S.
– ident: e_1_2_9_17_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2007.00602.x
– ident: e_1_2_9_56_1
  doi: 10.1037/0033‐295X.103.3.403
– ident: e_1_2_9_32_1
  doi: 10.1037/0096‐1523.33.4.816
– ident: e_1_2_9_64_1
  doi: 10.1037/0096‐1523.1.3.268
– ident: e_1_2_9_34_1
  doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213
– ident: e_1_2_9_59_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2010.00979.x
– ident: e_1_2_9_40_1
  doi: 10.1007/s00426‐008‐0211‐1
– ident: e_1_2_9_50_1
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0206468
– ident: e_1_2_9_5_1
  doi: 10.1016/0168‐5597(94)00256‐E
– ident: e_1_2_9_62_1
  doi: 10.1037/a0022014
– volume-title: Cogent 2000 [Computer software]
  year: 2003
  ident: e_1_2_9_7_1
  contributor:
    fullname: Cogent 2000 Team
– ident: e_1_2_9_8_1
  doi: 10.1007/978-1-4684-4178-9_11
– ident: e_1_2_9_23_1
– ident: e_1_2_9_52_1
  doi: 10.1007/s11065‐009‐9117‐y
– ident: e_1_2_9_37_1
  doi: 10.1037/0096‐1523.20.5.1000
– ident: e_1_2_9_39_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467‐8721.2007.00478.x
– ident: e_1_2_9_67_1
  doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00058015
– ident: e_1_2_9_69_1
  doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.378
– ident: e_1_2_9_65_1
  doi: 10.1111/ejn.12936
– ident: e_1_2_9_74_1
  doi: 10.1177/0956797620954815
– ident: e_1_2_9_26_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.1986.tb00649.x
– ident: e_1_2_9_43_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.07.032
– ident: e_1_2_9_24_1
  doi: 10.1080/713756001
SSID ssj0009122
Score 2.4833388
Snippet The contextual cueing effect is the phenomenon observed when response time (RT) becomes faster in visual search in repeated context compared with a new one. In...
SourceID proquest
crossref
pubmed
SourceType Aggregation Database
Index Database
StartPage e14361
SubjectTerms Age
Age groups
Decision making
Event-related potentials
Older people
Visual perception
Title Mechanisms of spatial contextual cueing in younger and older adults
URI https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37294010
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2877268702
https://search.proquest.com/docview/2824684730
Volume 60
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://sdu.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3dT9swELcKe9nLxGAfZQwZbdrLlMl2Etd9BFZgGuuQViS0l8hJbIg2kqppkbq_fuePfPCAxB72EiWXxK3ufjnfne_OCL0PTY8opXQgtNnCjIg0kELRQHLbvT2jNDMx3bMfo-mV-DyJJoNBk23R0f6rpIEGsjaVs_8g7XZQIMA5yByOIHU4Pkru35Sp5S3qW5ujUZuEadsCpDQpHqZWJFspX8eyNl-6ckmUldmt23XjqPsGq1OQNv5xLwD_tYJ_cyMX1Z30ab5Fq-Cn0tW_HFXXv-1CPF386hKC_hTXvvrwS728c_dbhJ5KR7CB6Z-1uTgKdSE_mkbP_QgFC32pnp1gvFaNSBCPXY_2Ru26bQQaeNGeElVgw3Ham5I7wgMKf16v5596b_W7ak-_JyeX5-fJbHI120BPGCgkow_D8KJrzkwZ861rTZZXN9p9Y-UBD8RaIrMt9My7EPjQyf45GqhyG-0clnJZ3a7xB3zRCmsHHXdwwJXGHg64gwN2cMBFiT0cMMABWzhgB4cX6PJkMjs-C_y-GUEWxnQZ5FEOZi_PdKqJkkSmgo9lKIgE31HEmmo-JqniI80ynomx0orxWKXwTAoKGfjzEm2WValeIyylgAkhYlkc8YjmuYSRBFEp5yyPYXoYoncNg5K5a4-SNG6lYWNi2ThEew3vEv-p1Am46iPGYbpgQ3TQ3gblZlasZKmqlXmGRRzsp5AM0SvH8_ZnzHpzRCjZfcTbb9DTDph7aHO5WKm3aKPOV_sWCX8Bzjx3Cg
link.rule.ids 315,782,786,27933,27934
linkProvider Wiley-Blackwell
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Mechanisms+of+spatial+contextual+cueing+in+younger+and+older+adults&rft.jtitle=Psychophysiology&rft.au=Kojouharova%2C+Petia&rft.au=Nagy%2C+Bogl%C3%A1rka&rft.au=Czigler%2C+Istv%C3%A1n&rft.au=Ga%C3%A1l%2C+Zs%C3%B3fia+Anna&rft.date=2023-11-01&rft.eissn=1540-5958&rft.volume=60&rft.issue=11&rft.spage=e14361&rft.epage=e14361&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111%2Fpsyp.14361&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0048-5772&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0048-5772&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0048-5772&client=summon