Mechanisms of spatial contextual cueing in younger and older adults
The contextual cueing effect is the phenomenon observed when response time (RT) becomes faster in visual search in repeated context compared with a new one. In the present study, we explored whether the mechanisms involved in the effect are age dependent. We investigated it in younger (N = 20, 12 wo...
Saved in:
Published in: | Psychophysiology Vol. 60; no. 11; p. e14361 |
---|---|
Main Authors: | , , , |
Format: | Journal Article |
Language: | English |
Published: |
United States
Blackwell Publishing Ltd
01-11-2023
|
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | Get full text |
Tags: |
Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
|
Abstract | The contextual cueing effect is the phenomenon observed when response time (RT) becomes faster in visual search in repeated context compared with a new one. In the present study, we explored whether the mechanisms involved in the effect are age dependent. We investigated it in younger (N = 20, 12 women, 21.2 ± 1.75 years) and older (N = 19, nine women, 67.05 ± 3.94 years) adults. We found a faster target identification in the repeated configurations with similar magnitude in the two age groups, which indicates that this contextual cueing effect remained intact even in the older participants. To shed light on the underlying mechanisms, we measured and compared the amplitude of three event-related potentials: N2pc, P3, and response-locked LRP. In the younger group, the larger contextual cueing effect (novel-minus-repeated RT difference) correlated positively with a larger difference in amplitude for repeated compared with novel configurations for both the N2pc and the P3 components, but there was no correlation with the response-locked lateralized readiness potential (rLRP) amplitude difference. However, in the older group, only the rLRP amplitude difference between novel and repeated configurations showed an enhancement with larger contextual cueing. These results suggest that different mechanisms are responsible for the contextual effect in the two age groups. It has both an early and an intermediate locus in younger adults: effective attentional allocation and successful stimulus categorization, or decision-making confidence are involved; while in older adults, a late locus was identified: a more efficient response organization led to a faster reaction. |
---|---|
AbstractList | The contextual cueing effect is the phenomenon observed when response time (RT) becomes faster in visual search in repeated context compared with a new one. In the present study, we explored whether the mechanisms involved in the effect are age dependent. We investigated it in younger (N = 20, 12 women, 21.2 ± 1.75 years) and older (N = 19, nine women, 67.05 ± 3.94 years) adults. We found a faster target identification in the repeated configurations with similar magnitude in the two age groups, which indicates that this contextual cueing effect remained intact even in the older participants. To shed light on the underlying mechanisms, we measured and compared the amplitude of three event‐related potentials: N2pc, P3, and response‐locked LRP. In the younger group, the larger contextual cueing effect (novel‐minus‐repeated RT difference) correlated positively with a larger difference in amplitude for repeated compared with novel configurations for both the N2pc and the P3 components, but there was no correlation with the response‐locked lateralized readiness potential (rLRP) amplitude difference. However, in the older group, only the rLRP amplitude difference between novel and repeated configurations showed an enhancement with larger contextual cueing. These results suggest that different mechanisms are responsible for the contextual effect in the two age groups. It has both an early and an intermediate locus in younger adults: effective attentional allocation and successful stimulus categorization, or decision‐making confidence are involved; while in older adults, a late locus was identified: a more efficient response organization led to a faster reaction. The contextual cueing effect is the phenomenon observed when response time (RT) becomes faster in visual search in repeated context compared with a new one. In the present study, we explored whether the mechanisms involved in the effect are age dependent. We investigated it in younger ( N = 20, 12 women, 21.2 ± 1.75 years) and older ( N = 19, nine women, 67.05 ± 3.94 years) adults. We found a faster target identification in the repeated configurations with similar magnitude in the two age groups, which indicates that this contextual cueing effect remained intact even in the older participants. To shed light on the underlying mechanisms, we measured and compared the amplitude of three event‐related potentials: N2pc, P3, and response‐locked LRP. In the younger group, the larger contextual cueing effect (novel‐minus‐repeated RT difference) correlated positively with a larger difference in amplitude for repeated compared with novel configurations for both the N2pc and the P3 components, but there was no correlation with the response‐locked lateralized readiness potential (rLRP) amplitude difference. However, in the older group, only the rLRP amplitude difference between novel and repeated configurations showed an enhancement with larger contextual cueing. These results suggest that different mechanisms are responsible for the contextual effect in the two age groups. It has both an early and an intermediate locus in younger adults: effective attentional allocation and successful stimulus categorization, or decision‐making confidence are involved; while in older adults, a late locus was identified: a more efficient response organization led to a faster reaction. In our study, we show that younger and older adults rely on different mechanisms when using contextual information to improve performance in a visual search task. Finding a target in repeated spatial configurations compared with new ones (contextual cueing effect) was equally faster in both age groups, which was a consequence of more efficient attention allocation and stimuli identification in younger and more efficient inhibition of distractor stimuli and response organization in older adults. |
Author | Kojouharova, Petia Nagy, Boglárka Czigler, István Gaál, Zsófia Anna |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Petia orcidid: 0000-0001-6315-4822 surname: Kojouharova fullname: Kojouharova, Petia organization: Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Budapest, Hungary – sequence: 2 givenname: Boglárka orcidid: 0000-0001-7897-839X surname: Nagy fullname: Nagy, Boglárka organization: Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Budapest, Hungary – sequence: 3 givenname: István orcidid: 0000-0003-1661-3808 surname: Czigler fullname: Czigler, István organization: Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Budapest, Hungary – sequence: 4 givenname: Zsófia Anna orcidid: 0000-0002-6615-5393 surname: Gaál fullname: Gaál, Zsófia Anna organization: Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Budapest, Hungary |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37294010$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNpd0MtKxDAUgOEgI85FNz6AFNyI0DG35rKUwRuMuNF1SdNk7NBJatOA8_amjrowm3MWH4fwz8HEeWcAOEdwidK76cK-WyJKGDoCM1RQmBeyEBMwg5CKvOAcT8E8hC2EUCKMT8CUcCwpRHAGVs9GvyvXhF3IvM1Cp4ZGtZn2bjCfQxzXaBq3yRqX7X10G9NnytWZb-txq2M7hFNwbFUbzNnPXIC3-7vX1WO-fnl4Wt2uc00KNOQ1rQWRTNvKQqOgqgSTigioJKeisMgyCSvDuMWaaSGNNZgVpkqmIkhySRbg6nC36_1HNGEod03Qpm2VMz6GEgtMmaCcwEQv_9Gtj71Lv0sqBWGCQ5zU9UHp3ofQG1t2fbNT_b5EsBzTlmPa8jttwhc_J2O1M_Uf_W1JvgCw9XXF |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhae175 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_02135 crossref_primary_10_3389_fcogn_2024_1336379 crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218241241954 |
Cites_doi | 10.1037/xhp0000394 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.022 10.1080/13506285.2019.1668516 10.3758/s13415‐016‐0442‐3 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2007.00611.x 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.1977.tb01312.x 10.1093/geronb/62.2.P78 10.1126/science.172.3990.1357 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2009.00807.x 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.12.012 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.011 10.1006/cogp.1998.0681 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.021 10.1111/psyp.13542 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 10.1017/S0140525X00058027 10.3758/BF03193146 10.1093/brain/awh042 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.05.012 10.1037/cep0000007 10.3758/BF03206196 10.1027//0269‐8803.16.3.167 10.3389/fnhum.2020.569614 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.02.007 10.1016/S0168‐5597(97)96139‐6 10.1037/0882‐7974.17.1.85 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.06.004 10.1016/0168‐5597(88)90019‐6 10.3758/s13414‐019‐01832‐2 10.1093/cercor/bhm155 10.1016/0013‐4694(96)95711‐9 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.1980.tb00131.x 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.03.025 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2005.00285.x 10.1037/0894‐4105.18.1.124 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.009 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.02.025 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.09.011 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.003 10.3758/BF03194108 10.3758/BF03212388 10.1111/psyp.12063 10.1037/h0020586 10.3389/fnagi.2020.596047 10.1007/s11065‐014‐9270‐9 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.08.003 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2007.00602.x 10.1037/0033‐295X.103.3.403 10.1037/0096‐1523.33.4.816 10.1037/0096‐1523.1.3.268 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2010.00979.x 10.1007/s00426‐008‐0211‐1 10.1371/journal.pone.0206468 10.1016/0168‐5597(94)00256‐E 10.1037/a0022014 10.1007/978-1-4684-4178-9_11 10.1007/s11065‐009‐9117‐y 10.1037/0096‐1523.20.5.1000 10.1111/j.1467‐8721.2007.00478.x 10.1017/S0140525X00058015 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.378 10.1111/ejn.12936 10.1177/0956797620954815 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.1986.tb00649.x 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.07.032 10.1080/713756001 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2023 The Authors. Psychophysiology published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of Society for Psychophysiological Research. 2023. This article is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2023 The Authors. Psychophysiology published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of Society for Psychophysiological Research. – notice: 2023. This article is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. |
DBID | NPM AAYXX CITATION 7TK K9. 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1111/psyp.14361 |
DatabaseName | PubMed CrossRef Neurosciences Abstracts ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | PubMed CrossRef ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni) PubMed CrossRef |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anatomy & Physiology |
EISSN | 1540-5958 1469-8986 |
EndPage | e14361 |
ExternalDocumentID | 10_1111_psyp_14361 37294010 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Hungarian Scientific Research Fund grantid: OTKA K 132880 |
GroupedDBID | --- --Z -DZ -~X .3N .GA 05W 0R~ 10A 123 1OB 1OC 29P 2QV 33P 36B 4.4 50Y 50Z 51W 51Y 52M 52O 52Q 52R 52S 52T 52U 52V 52W 5HH 5LA 5RE 5VS 66C 702 7PT 8-0 8-1 8-3 8-4 8-5 8UM 930 A01 A04 AABNI AAESR AAHHS AAONW AAOUF AAXRX AAZKR ABCQN ABCUV ABDBF ABGDZ ABITZ ABIVO ABJNI ABLJU ABPPZ ABPVW ABQWH ABSOO ABXGK ACAHQ ACBKW ACCFJ ACCZN ACFBH ACGFO ACGFS ACGOF ACHQT ACMXC ACNCT ACPOU ACPRK ACWUS ACXQS ADBBV ADBTR ADEMA ADEOM ADIZJ ADKYN ADMGS ADXAS ADZMN AEEZP AEGXH AEIGN AEIMD AENEX AEQDE AEUQT AEUYR AFBPY AFEBI AFFPM AFGKR AFKFF AFPWT AFZJQ AHBTC AHMBA AIACR AIAGR AIFKG AIURR AIWBW AJBDE ALAGY ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALUQN AMBMR AMYDB ASTYK AZBYB AZVAB BAFTC BDRZF BFHJK BMXJE BNVMJ BQESF BROTX BRXPI BY8 CS3 D-6 D-7 D-C D-D DCZOG DPXWK DR2 DRFUL DRMAN DRSSH DU5 DXH EAD EAP EBC EBD EBS EMB EMK EMOBN EPS ESX F00 F01 F5P FUBAC G-S G.N G50 GODZA HGLYW HZI HZ~ IHE IX1 J0M K48 KBYEO L7B LATKE LC2 LC4 LEEKS LH4 LITHE LOXES LP6 LP7 LUTES LYRES MEWTI MK4 MRFUL MRMAN MRSSH MSFUL MSMAN MSSSH MXFUL MXMAN MXSSH N04 N06 N9A NF~ NPM O66 O9- OIG P2W P2Y P2Z P4B P4C PQQKQ Q.N Q11 QB0 R.K RCA RIG ROL RX1 S10 SUPJJ SV3 TN5 TUS UB1 V8K VQA W8V W99 WBKPD WH7 WHDPE WIH WII WIJ WOHZO WQZ WRC WSUWO WXI WXSBR XG1 XSW YNT YYM YYP YZZ ZCA ZZTAW ~IA ~WP .55 .GJ .Y3 186 31~ 53G AAMNL AASGY AAYJJ AAYXX ABDPE ABEML ACBWZ ACQPF ACSCC AFFNX AHEFC ASPBG AVWKF AZFZN CAG CITATION COF DC6 EJD FEDTE FZ0 HAOEW HF~ HVGLF LPU LW6 MVM NEJ OHT OVD PALCI RIWAO RJQFR SAMSI TEORI UAP X7M XJT XOL ZGI ZWS ZXP 7TK K9. 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c351t-d4d8396cfbf0ea0ab869a380a97485f1f690be67f2c6c89efe265eb9a3b319793 |
ISSN | 0048-5772 |
IngestDate | Sat Oct 05 05:41:53 EDT 2024 Thu Oct 10 15:51:12 EDT 2024 Thu Nov 21 23:41:33 EST 2024 Sat Sep 28 08:11:41 EDT 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 11 |
Keywords | ERP contextual cueing N2pc P3 LRP aging |
Language | English |
License | 2023 The Authors. Psychophysiology published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of Society for Psychophysiological Research. |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c351t-d4d8396cfbf0ea0ab869a380a97485f1f690be67f2c6c89efe265eb9a3b319793 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0003-1661-3808 0000-0002-6615-5393 0000-0001-6315-4822 0000-0001-7897-839X |
OpenAccessLink | https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdfdirect/10.1111/psyp.14361 |
PMID | 37294010 |
PQID | 2877268702 |
PQPubID | 41359 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_2824684730 proquest_journals_2877268702 crossref_primary_10_1111_psyp_14361 pubmed_primary_37294010 |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2023-11-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2023-11-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 11 year: 2023 text: 2023-11-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States – name: Oxford |
PublicationTitle | Psychophysiology |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Psychophysiology |
PublicationYear | 2023 |
Publisher | Blackwell Publishing Ltd |
Publisher_xml | – name: Blackwell Publishing Ltd |
References | e_1_2_9_31_1 e_1_2_9_52_1 e_1_2_9_50_1 e_1_2_9_73_1 e_1_2_9_10_1 e_1_2_9_35_1 e_1_2_9_56_1 e_1_2_9_12_1 e_1_2_9_33_1 e_1_2_9_71_1 e_1_2_9_14_1 e_1_2_9_39_1 e_1_2_9_16_1 e_1_2_9_37_1 e_1_2_9_58_1 e_1_2_9_18_1 e_1_2_9_41_1 e_1_2_9_64_1 e_1_2_9_20_1 e_1_2_9_62_1 e_1_2_9_22_1 e_1_2_9_45_1 e_1_2_9_68_1 e_1_2_9_24_1 e_1_2_9_43_1 e_1_2_9_66_1 e_1_2_9_8_1 Luck S. J. (e_1_2_9_36_1) 1990; 16 e_1_2_9_6_1 e_1_2_9_4_1 e_1_2_9_60_1 e_1_2_9_2_1 e_1_2_9_26_1 e_1_2_9_49_1 e_1_2_9_28_1 e_1_2_9_47_1 e_1_2_9_30_1 e_1_2_9_53_1 e_1_2_9_74_1 e_1_2_9_51_1 e_1_2_9_72_1 e_1_2_9_11_1 e_1_2_9_34_1 e_1_2_9_57_1 e_1_2_9_13_1 e_1_2_9_32_1 Cogent 2000 Team (e_1_2_9_7_1) 2003 e_1_2_9_70_1 e_1_2_9_15_1 e_1_2_9_38_1 Rózsa S. (e_1_2_9_55_1) 2010 e_1_2_9_17_1 e_1_2_9_59_1 e_1_2_9_19_1 e_1_2_9_42_1 e_1_2_9_63_1 e_1_2_9_40_1 e_1_2_9_61_1 e_1_2_9_21_1 e_1_2_9_46_1 e_1_2_9_67_1 e_1_2_9_23_1 e_1_2_9_44_1 e_1_2_9_65_1 e_1_2_9_5_1 e_1_2_9_3_1 Romaya J. (e_1_2_9_54_1) 2008 e_1_2_9_9_1 e_1_2_9_25_1 e_1_2_9_27_1 e_1_2_9_48_1 e_1_2_9_69_1 e_1_2_9_29_1 |
References_xml | – ident: e_1_2_9_20_1 doi: 10.1037/xhp0000394 – ident: e_1_2_9_66_1 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.022 – ident: e_1_2_9_49_1 doi: 10.1080/13506285.2019.1668516 – ident: e_1_2_9_31_1 doi: 10.3758/s13415‐016‐0442‐3 – ident: e_1_2_9_29_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2007.00611.x – ident: e_1_2_9_12_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.1977.tb01312.x – ident: e_1_2_9_53_1 doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.2.P78 – volume-title: Cogent graphics [Computer software] year: 2008 ident: e_1_2_9_54_1 contributor: fullname: Romaya J. – ident: e_1_2_9_21_1 doi: 10.1126/science.172.3990.1357 – ident: e_1_2_9_58_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2009.00807.x – ident: e_1_2_9_48_1 doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.12.012 – ident: e_1_2_9_72_1 doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.011 – ident: e_1_2_9_6_1 doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0681 – ident: e_1_2_9_44_1 doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.021 – ident: e_1_2_9_68_1 doi: 10.1111/psyp.13542 – ident: e_1_2_9_10_1 doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 – ident: e_1_2_9_11_1 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00058027 – ident: e_1_2_9_16_1 doi: 10.3758/BF03193146 – ident: e_1_2_9_73_1 doi: 10.1093/brain/awh042 – ident: e_1_2_9_42_1 doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.05.012 – ident: e_1_2_9_38_1 doi: 10.1037/cep0000007 – ident: e_1_2_9_45_1 doi: 10.3758/BF03206196 – ident: e_1_2_9_15_1 doi: 10.1027//0269‐8803.16.3.167 – ident: e_1_2_9_18_1 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.569614 – ident: e_1_2_9_35_1 doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.02.007 – ident: e_1_2_9_47_1 doi: 10.1016/S0168‐5597(97)96139‐6 – ident: e_1_2_9_3_1 doi: 10.1037/0882‐7974.17.1.85 – ident: e_1_2_9_41_1 – ident: e_1_2_9_2_1 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.06.004 – ident: e_1_2_9_46_1 doi: 10.1016/0168‐5597(88)90019‐6 – ident: e_1_2_9_61_1 doi: 10.3758/s13414‐019‐01832‐2 – ident: e_1_2_9_9_1 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm155 – ident: e_1_2_9_13_1 doi: 10.1016/0013‐4694(96)95711‐9 – ident: e_1_2_9_27_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.1980.tb00131.x – ident: e_1_2_9_71_1 doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.03.025 – ident: e_1_2_9_14_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2005.00285.x – ident: e_1_2_9_22_1 doi: 10.1037/0894‐4105.18.1.124 – ident: e_1_2_9_19_1 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.009 – ident: e_1_2_9_33_1 doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.02.025 – ident: e_1_2_9_70_1 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.09.011 – ident: e_1_2_9_57_1 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.003 – ident: e_1_2_9_25_1 doi: 10.3758/BF03194108 – ident: e_1_2_9_63_1 doi: 10.3758/BF03212388 – ident: e_1_2_9_4_1 doi: 10.1111/psyp.12063 – ident: e_1_2_9_60_1 doi: 10.1037/h0020586 – ident: e_1_2_9_30_1 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.596047 – ident: e_1_2_9_51_1 doi: 10.1007/s11065‐014‐9270‐9 – volume: 16 start-page: 577 year: 1990 ident: e_1_2_9_36_1 article-title: Evidence for an ERP correlate of attentional filtering publication-title: Society for Neuroscience Abstracts contributor: fullname: Luck S. J. – ident: e_1_2_9_28_1 doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.08.003 – volume-title: WAIS‐IV. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition year: 2010 ident: e_1_2_9_55_1 contributor: fullname: Rózsa S. – ident: e_1_2_9_17_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2007.00602.x – ident: e_1_2_9_56_1 doi: 10.1037/0033‐295X.103.3.403 – ident: e_1_2_9_32_1 doi: 10.1037/0096‐1523.33.4.816 – ident: e_1_2_9_64_1 doi: 10.1037/0096‐1523.1.3.268 – ident: e_1_2_9_34_1 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213 – ident: e_1_2_9_59_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.2010.00979.x – ident: e_1_2_9_40_1 doi: 10.1007/s00426‐008‐0211‐1 – ident: e_1_2_9_50_1 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0206468 – ident: e_1_2_9_5_1 doi: 10.1016/0168‐5597(94)00256‐E – ident: e_1_2_9_62_1 doi: 10.1037/a0022014 – volume-title: Cogent 2000 [Computer software] year: 2003 ident: e_1_2_9_7_1 contributor: fullname: Cogent 2000 Team – ident: e_1_2_9_8_1 doi: 10.1007/978-1-4684-4178-9_11 – ident: e_1_2_9_23_1 – ident: e_1_2_9_52_1 doi: 10.1007/s11065‐009‐9117‐y – ident: e_1_2_9_37_1 doi: 10.1037/0096‐1523.20.5.1000 – ident: e_1_2_9_39_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467‐8721.2007.00478.x – ident: e_1_2_9_67_1 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00058015 – ident: e_1_2_9_69_1 doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.378 – ident: e_1_2_9_65_1 doi: 10.1111/ejn.12936 – ident: e_1_2_9_74_1 doi: 10.1177/0956797620954815 – ident: e_1_2_9_26_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐8986.1986.tb00649.x – ident: e_1_2_9_43_1 doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.07.032 – ident: e_1_2_9_24_1 doi: 10.1080/713756001 |
SSID | ssj0009122 |
Score | 2.4833388 |
Snippet | The contextual cueing effect is the phenomenon observed when response time (RT) becomes faster in visual search in repeated context compared with a new one. In... |
SourceID | proquest crossref pubmed |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database |
StartPage | e14361 |
SubjectTerms | Age Age groups Decision making Event-related potentials Older people Visual perception |
Title | Mechanisms of spatial contextual cueing in younger and older adults |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37294010 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2877268702 https://search.proquest.com/docview/2824684730 |
Volume | 60 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://sdu.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV3dT9swELcKe9nLxGAfZQwZbdrLlMl2Etd9BFZgGuuQViS0l8hJbIg2kqppkbq_fuePfPCAxB72EiWXxK3ufjnfne_OCL0PTY8opXQgtNnCjIg0kELRQHLbvT2jNDMx3bMfo-mV-DyJJoNBk23R0f6rpIEGsjaVs_8g7XZQIMA5yByOIHU4Pkru35Sp5S3qW5ujUZuEadsCpDQpHqZWJFspX8eyNl-6ckmUldmt23XjqPsGq1OQNv5xLwD_tYJ_cyMX1Z30ab5Fq-Cn0tW_HFXXv-1CPF386hKC_hTXvvrwS728c_dbhJ5KR7CB6Z-1uTgKdSE_mkbP_QgFC32pnp1gvFaNSBCPXY_2Ru26bQQaeNGeElVgw3Ham5I7wgMKf16v5596b_W7ak-_JyeX5-fJbHI120BPGCgkow_D8KJrzkwZ861rTZZXN9p9Y-UBD8RaIrMt9My7EPjQyf45GqhyG-0clnJZ3a7xB3zRCmsHHXdwwJXGHg64gwN2cMBFiT0cMMABWzhgB4cX6PJkMjs-C_y-GUEWxnQZ5FEOZi_PdKqJkkSmgo9lKIgE31HEmmo-JqniI80ynomx0orxWKXwTAoKGfjzEm2WValeIyylgAkhYlkc8YjmuYSRBFEp5yyPYXoYoncNg5K5a4-SNG6lYWNi2ThEew3vEv-p1Am46iPGYbpgQ3TQ3gblZlasZKmqlXmGRRzsp5AM0SvH8_ZnzHpzRCjZfcTbb9DTDph7aHO5WKm3aKPOV_sWCX8Bzjx3Cg |
link.rule.ids | 315,782,786,27933,27934 |
linkProvider | Wiley-Blackwell |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Mechanisms+of+spatial+contextual+cueing+in+younger+and+older+adults&rft.jtitle=Psychophysiology&rft.au=Kojouharova%2C+Petia&rft.au=Nagy%2C+Bogl%C3%A1rka&rft.au=Czigler%2C+Istv%C3%A1n&rft.au=Ga%C3%A1l%2C+Zs%C3%B3fia+Anna&rft.date=2023-11-01&rft.eissn=1540-5958&rft.volume=60&rft.issue=11&rft.spage=e14361&rft.epage=e14361&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111%2Fpsyp.14361&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0048-5772&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0048-5772&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0048-5772&client=summon |